Consider your future practice as a counselor, keeping in mind a population and/or setting for your work. The Final Course Project is a synthesis of literature and it is aimed to help you determine the

Infant and Child Development Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) Published online 30 July 2008 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/icd.578 Relationships among Parenting Practices, Parental Stress, Child Behaviour, and Children’s Social- Cognitive Development Nicole R. Guajardo a, , Gregory Snyder band Rachel Petersen c aDepartment of Psychology, Christopher Newport University, Newport News, VA, USA bChildren’s Hospital, Omaha, NE, USAcPsychological Assessment Specialists, Pocatello, ID, USA The present study included observational and self-report measures to examine associations among parental stress, parental behaviour, child behaviour, and children’s theory of mind and emotion understanding. Eighty-three parents and their 3- to 5-year-old children participated. Parents completed measures of parental stress, parenting (laxness, overreactivity), and child behaviour (internalizing, externalizing); children completed language, theory of mind, and emotion understanding measures. Parent–child interactions also were observed (N547). Laxness and parenting stress predicted children’s theory of mind performance and parental usage of imitative gestures and vocalizations accounted for unique variance in emotion understanding. Associations also were found between child behaviour and emotion understanding.

Results provide support for direct and indirect associations between parent–child interactions and early social-cognitive development. Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Key words:parenting; theory of mind; emotion; parental stress INTRODUCTION Two important aspects of early social-cognitive development are theory of mind and emotion understanding. By 4–5 years of age children typically understand the representational nature of thoughts and beliefs (Wellman, Cross, & Watson, *Correspondence to: Nicole R. Guajardo, Department of Psychology, Christopher Newport University, 1 University Place, Newport News, VA 23693, USA. E-mail: [email protected] Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 2001) and the causes of different emotions (Denham & Couchoud, 1990), yet there is variability among children. Consistent with Vygotsky’s (1978) theory, one factor that can in uence when and how well children understand such concepts is the nature of interactions with others, particularly parents (see Astington, 1996; Hughes, Deater-Deckard, & Cutting, 1999; Ruffman, Perner, & Parkin, 1999). Indeed, attachment (e.g. Arranz, Artamendi, Olabarrieta, & Martin, 2002; Fonagy, Redfern, & Charman, 1997; Symons & Clark, 2000), parent–child conversations (e.g. Brown, Donelan-McCall, & Dunn, 1996; Dunn, Brown, & Beardsall, 1991; Meins & Fernyhough, 1999), parenting style (Vinden, 2001), and parental approaches to discipline (Hugheset al., 1999; Ruffmanet al., 1999) all have been found to relate to children’s theory of mind and/or emotion understanding.

Variables outside the immediate parent–child relationship also might account for individual differences in early social-cognitive performance. According to family systems theory (Minuchin & Fishman, 1981), broader factors that in uence the parent also can affect the dynamics between parents and children. Working within these theoretical frameworks, the present study integrated clinical and developmental methods to assess relationships among parental stress, parental behaviours, and children’s outcomes. More speci cally, we examined whether parental stress relates to parental responsivity and discipline styles, and whether these in turn predict children’s theory of mind and emotion understanding. We also examined whether theory of mind and emotion understanding then relate to children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviours. Each of these factors is discussed in turn.

Parental Stress Family systems theories as well as Bronfenbrenner ’s (1979, 2006) ecological systems approach emphasize family functioning as a major force contributing to adaptive, and maladaptive, child development. Parenting stress is one of many factors, related to parents’ parenting approaches and effectiveness. Parents experiencing high levels of stress, particularly from economic dif culties, typically are less responsive and affectionate with their children and more likely to use power-assertive techniques, as compared with parents without such stress (see McLoyd, 1990). It follows that excessive parental stress, through its effect on parenting, could negatively affect a child’s social-cognitive development; however, two studies have yielded mixed results. Cole and Mitchell (1998) observed an inverse relationship between parental stress and children’s performances on false belief tasks. In contrast, Symons and Clark (2002) observed that mothers’ self- reported emotional distress when their child was 2 years old positively predicted their child’s performance on a caregiver location task at 5 years of age. Differing methods likely played a role in the contradictory results; however, both studies have begun to explore systematically these complex family relationships.

Parenting, Theory of Mind, and Emotion Parenting is directly related to theory of mind and emotion understanding. In particular, research related to theory of mind has examined the importance of such variables as attachment, parenting style, and discipline. Bowlby (1982) suggested that secure attachments enable children to learn about their caretaker ’s perspective, which then allows a child to recognize differences between his/her N.R. Guajardo et al. 38 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd own and his/her caretaker ’s perspectives. During the fourth stage of attachment children become able to attribute different thoughts and feelings to themselves and others (see Humfress, O’Connor, Slaughter, Target, & Fonagy, 2002). Recent theory of mind and emotion research has supported Bowlby’s theory about links between attachment and children’s mentalizing (e.g. Arranzet al., 2002; Fonagyet al., 1997; Laible & Thompson, 1998; Meins, Fernyhough, Russel, & Clark-Carter, 1998; Symons & Clark, 2000). In fact, attachment classi cation in infancy predicts false belief performance in the preschool years (Meinset al., 1998; Symons & Clark, 2000).

A central characteristic of secure parent–child attachments is responsive par- enting. Mothers with secure attachments are sensitive tutors, meaning they modify their instructions appropriately in reaction to their children’s behaviour (Meins, 1997; Meinset al., 1998). Responsive parenting also involves re ecting on a child’s motives and mental states (Symons & Clark, 2002). Indeed, parents’ tendencies to treat their children as having intentions and mental states at 6 months positively predicts theory of mind scores at 4 years (Meinset al., 1998).

Such ndings suggest that sensitive parents attend to their children’s mental states and re ect them appropriately. Such re ection, in turn, provides oppor- tunities for children to learn about mental perspectives.

Parental control and discipline style also are related to theory of mind and emotion development. In particular, parental control in general (Vinden, 2001), parents’ negative control during discipline (e.g. criticism or physical control of child; Hugheset al., 1999), and power assertion (spanking, yelling; Pears & Moses, 2003) are inversely related to children’s theory of mind performance. These nd- ings t with Vinden’s (2001) explanation that parents who use controlling tech- niques do not provide opportunities for children to learn about others’ perspectives. Interestingly, Pears and Moses found that children whose parents reported use of power assertion performed better on measures of emotion un- derstanding. It is possible that the strong affect associated with power assertion leads to an emphasis on others’ emotions and the fact that people can have dif- ferent feelings about the same situation (Pears & Moses, 2003). The nding that the same parental behaviour can have differential effects on various aspects of social cognition is consistent with previous work that indicated that theory of mind and emotion understanding are overlapping, yet unique constructs (see Cutting & Dunn, 1999; Weimer & Guajardo, 2005).

One relevant moderator for the effect of parenting on theory of mind devel- opment is sex. Hugheset al. (1999) found that associations between parenting and theory of mind differ for boys and girls. Boys’ theory of mind performance related positively to severity of parental discipline, while girls’ performance re- lated positively to general parental warmth. These ndings could re ect mean- ingful differences in how girls and boys use their theory of mind knowledge, such that girls use their understanding of mind to foster relationships with an emphasis on emotional support, empathy, and cooperation, while boys use their knowledge in less prosocial ways, leading to reasons for severe discipline (Hugheset al., 1999). The present study examined relationships among re- sponsive parenting, theory of mind, and emotion understanding and whether such relationships differed for boys and girls.

Social Competence and Peer Interactions In addition to factors that predict children’s social-cognitive skills, research also has shown links between these skills and children’s outcomes, particularly social Parenting and Social Cognition39 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd competence (e.g. Capage & Watson, 2001; Lalonde & Chandler, 1995). Typically, children rated by teachers and/or parents as socially skilled perform well on theory of mind tasks (Capage & Watson, 2001; Lalonde & Chandler, 1995; Watson, Nixon, Wilson, & Capage, 1999). Such children are more likely to demonstrate intentional behaviours (e.g. commenting on differences between one’s own and another ’s wishes) during social interactions (Lalonde & Chandler, 1995), possess positive social skills, and vocalize more frequently with peers during play (Watsonet al., 1999). They also are less likely to be socially withdrawn (Badenes, Estevan, & Bacete, 2000). Similarly, children’s theory of mind and emotion understanding predict their peer likeability (Cassidy, Werner, Rourke, Zubernis, & Balaraman, 2003), and emotion regulation is linked with peer acceptance (Raver, Blackburn, Bancroft, & Torp, 1999). Children’s understanding of others’ thoughts, beliefs, and feelings might increase the likelihood of them solving social problems in appropriate ways rather than being aggressive (Capage & Watson, 2001).

Children perceived as lacking social competence perform less well on social- cognitive measures. For example, hard-to-manage children perform less well on theory of mind and emotion understanding tasks than do children without be- haviour management issues (Hughes, Dunn, & White, 1998). Similarly, Happe and Frith (1996) found that though 6–12 year olds with conduct disorder passed standard rst-order tasks, they performed less well than controls on a set of everyday behaviours thought to involve mentalizing skills (e.g. responding to hints/indirect cues in conversation, initiating conversation of interest to others).

The previously mentioned study suggests that children with a better under- standing of others’ thoughts and feelings will be more socially competent than other children. Sutton, Smith, and Swettenham (1999a), in contrast, suggested that bullies actually might have high theory of mind understanding. Indeed, social intelligence is related to multiple types of aggression (Bjorkqvist, Osterman, & Kaukiainen, 2000). In particular, those who use indirect methods of bullying (e.g.

spreading rumours) need to be quite skilled at manipulating the thoughts of others (Suttonet al., 1999a). Such ndings suggest that theory of mind perfor- mance is not always linked with desirable behaviours (see also Sutton, Reeves, & Keogh, 2000; Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham, 1999b). Thus, the present study ex- plored whether theory of mind and emotion understanding are related to mala- daptive child outcomes, speci cally externalizing and internalizing behaviours.

Purpose The purpose of the present study was to examine associations between parental stress and parental behaviours (i.e. responsiveness, discipline) and then how those variables predicted theory of mind and emotion understanding. Finally, we also examined associations between these variables and children’s behavioural outcomes, internalizing and externalizing. Previous studies have explored several of these associations, but not within a single study. Moreover, this study brought together developmental and clinical approaches to the study of parenting and child outcomes.

Three sets of hypotheses were tested. First, the present study examined associations between parental stress and parental behaviours as well as parental stress and children’s theory of mind and emotion understanding. Given past ndings, we hypothesized that increases in parental stress would be nega- tively associated with aspects of parental responsivity and positively associated with negative aspects of parental behaviour (i.e. intrusiveness). Thus, parenting N.R. Guajardo et al. 40 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd responsivity and intrusiveness would mediate the relationship between parent- ing stress and children’s theory of mind and emotion understanding.

Second, the present study examined associations between parenting and chil- dren’s social-cognitive performance using both questionnaire and observational methods. Questionnaires assessed lax and overreactive parenting. These dimen- sions were selected because they are dimensions of parenting with clinical im- plications (Arnold, O’Leary, Wolff, & Acker, 1993) that are yet to be explored in the developmental literature. Overreactive parenting is characterized by harsh verbal commands and physical punishment, whereas laxness involves submitting to child demands. Consistent with previous research on parental control (Hugheset al., 1999; Pears & Moses, 2003), overreactive parenting was expected to be inversely related to theory of mind and positively related to emotion understanding. Given that previous study has not examined lax parenting in relation to children’s social- cognitive development, no speci c hypothesis was proposed.

Parental positive attention and ‘responsiveness’ also were examined. In this study, responsiveness was conceptualized as contingent reciprocation of a child’s verbal and non-verbal behaviour. Extending developmental research, the present study incorporated current clinical knowledge of disruptive children and their parents. All current empirically supported interventions (i.e. Helping the Non-compliant Child, Parent–Child Interaction Therapy, The Incredible Years) begin treatment by improving parental responsiveness during parent–child interactions by encouraging parents to increase the ‘non- demanding’ aspects of their attention during a speci ed period of play each day, including imitation (either physical or verbal), descriptions (labelling of their child’s play behaviour), and praise, while refraining from asking ques- tions of their child, criticizing their child’s play, or giving instructions/sug- gestions (Forehand & McMahon, 1981; Hembree-Kigin & McNeil, 1995; McMahon & Forehand, 2004; Webster-Stratton, 2000). In light of recent in- vestigations of links between parental responsiveness and children’s social- cognitive development, we hypothesized that theory of mind and emotion understanding would be positively related to positive aspects of parent be- haviour designed to increase child socialization and compliance and nega- tively correlated with those aspects of parent behaviour that increase children’s inappropriate and de ant behaviour.

The third set of hypotheses related to child outcome variables. More speci cally, we examined relationships between parental reports of child externalizing beha- viour and children’s theory of mind and emotion understanding. Consistent with preschool data (e.g. Capage & Watson, 2001; Hugheset al., 1998), we hypothesized that externalizing behaviour would be negatively related to both theory of mind and emotion understanding. Consistent with the work of Hugheset al. (1999), we examined whether these relationships depended upon the sex of the child.

METHOD Participants Eighty-three parents (65 mothers, 18 fathers;Mage528 years; range520–46) participated with their 3- to 5-year-old child (49 boys, 34 girls; age range536–71 months;M551 months). Observational data were able to be coded for 47 of the 83 parent–child dyads because of technical dif culties. The sample with observational data included 35 mothers and 12 fathers (Mage527.83; Parenting and Social Cognition41 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd range520–40) and 28 sons and 19 daughters (36–71 months;Mage550.4). To ensure that the two samples were comparable, group comparisons were completed for all central variables. Child completed emotion understanding (t50.56,p50.58) and theory of mind (t50.01,p50.99) scores were statistically equivalent between groups with and without available observational data.

Similarly, scores between groups on self-reported parenting measures [Parenting Scale (PS) overreactivet51.47,p50.15, and laxt51.65,p50.10] and child behaviour scales [Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) externalizingt5 0.11, p50.91, and internalizingt5 0.24,p50.81] were similar. Participants were solicited from a small, Northwestern city via the newspaper and introductory psychology courses. Parents received either $20.00 or research credit; children received a book of their choice.

Ninety-three per cent of all responding parents were Caucasian, 4% were Hispanic, 1% was American Indian, and 2% were Asian. Fifty-eight per cent of parents completed high school or the equivalent, 16% completed vocational or technical school, 9% completed an Associate’s degree, 13% completed a Bache- lor ’s degree, and 4% completed a Master ’s or professional degree. Mean household income was between $30 000–$40 000 (rangep$10 000–$90 000) and children had an average of 1.4 siblings (range from 0 to 5).

Sample characteristics based upon parental responses on the CBCL and the Parenting Stress Index were largely consistent with characteristics expected in a community-based sample. Generally, children and their parents were well ad- justed, with most not experiencing any major behavioural or emotional distress (see Table 1). Parental-reported child internalizing (i.e. depression, anxiety, so- matic complaints) behaviour were average, as was child externalizing (i.e. at- tention problems and rule breaking behaviour) and total emotional and behavioural problems. Moreover, parents generally indicated that they were not experiencing signi cant parent- or child-related stress.

Design Overview Parent–child dyads participated in one 1 1 2–2-h session at a university laboratory.

Sessions included two parts. During the rst part of the session, parents Table 1. Descriptive statistics for child and parent measures Mean S.D. Range Child-report measures Language comprehension 58 24.4 21–112 Theory of mind 3.1 1.7 0–7 Emotion understanding 1.86 0.74 0.06–3.19 Parent-report measures CBCL internalizingTscore 49.7 9.1 29–68 CBCL externalizingTscore 49.2 9.3 28–73 CBCL total problemTscore 49.6 9.2 29–76 Parenting Scale: laxness 2.6 0.8 1.1–4.7 Parenting Scale: overreactivity 3.2 0.9 1–5.6 Parenting Stress Index: parent related a 45.7 29 1.5–98 Parenting Stress Index: child related a 41.5 29.5 2.5–99 aParenting stress index scores are calculated percentages based on sample norms described in the administration manual (Abidin, 1995). N.R. Guajardo et al. 42 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd completed questionnaires concerning parenting stress, parenting practices, and child behaviour. In an adjacent room, children completed language, theory of mind, and emotion understanding tasks. The theory of mind and emotion understanding tasks were counterbalanced. The second part of the session consisted of three 10-min videotaped parent–child interactions: a free-play interaction, a parent-busy task, and a clean-up task.

Parent Questionnaires Demographic survey: Parents completed a demographic survey on which they indicated their and the other parent’s age, ethnicity, marital status, highest education level completed, and employment status. They also indicated the yearly household income, number and ages of children, and the number of adults living in the home. Yearly household income was indicated on an 11-point scale ranging from less than $10 000 to over $100 000. Entwisle and Astone (1994) recommended using nancial, human, and social capital as indicators of children’s socioeconomic status; thus, data on household income, parental education level, and number of adults in the household were collected.

Child problem behaviours: Children’s internalizing and externalizing problem behaviours were assessed via parental report using the CBCL 1.5–5 years (Achenbach, 1991). Parents responded to questions about different aspects of their child’s behaviour on a three-point scale (e.g. never, sometimes, and always).

The CBCL yields two broad measures of externalizing behaviour (i.e. de- linquency, aggressiveness) and internalizing behaviour (i.e. withdrawn, depressed). The CBCL is well researched and widely used, and the Externalizing Disorder Scale demonstrates adequate internal consistency, test–retest reliability, and concurrent validity with other measures of children’s conduct problems (Achenbach, 1995). Raw scores were converted toTscores for all analyses.

Parenting: Parenting approaches were examined with The Parenting Scale (PS; Arnoldet al., 1993), a 30-item scale that assesses parenting responses to child misbehaviour. For each item parents indicated on a Likert-type scale which of two responses was more characteristic of them. For example, for the rst item, ‘When my child misbehavesy’, they chose between ‘I do something right away’ and ‘I do something about it later ’. Two factors encompassing overreactivity (e.g.

When I am upset or under stress I am picky on my child’s back) and laxness (e.g. I threaten to do things that I know I won’t actually do) have demonstrated adequate reliability and concurrent validity with observed measures of parenting behaviour (Reitman, Currier, & Hupp, 2001). Both factors have acceptable internal consistency (alphas50.71 and 0.77, respectively) and distinguish clinic from non-clinic samples.

Parenting stress: Parents completed the Parenting Stress Index (Abidin, 1995) to assess the effect of stress on parent–child interactions. This 120-item measure is appropriate for parents of children between 1 month and 12 years of age. For each item parents either indicate the degree to which they agree with a statement on a ve-point scale, select the appropriate response from those provided, or respond yes or no to a question. Two composites, child domain and parent do- main, are calculated based upon 13 subscales. The child domain re ects the degree to which a parent perceives characteristics of her/his child as stressful (e.g. ‘My child does a few things that bother me a great deal’). The parent domain re ects stress related to the parents’ functioning (e.g. ‘I feel trapped by my re- sponsibilities as a parent’.). A third component of the scale, life stress, re ects the Parenting and Social Cognition43 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd degree of stress a parent feels outside the parent–child relationship (Abidin, 1995). Life stress is calculated based upon whether any of the 19 events have occurred within the previous 12 months (e.g. marriage, separation, death of an immediate family member). All three of these scores were used in the present study to determine the degree to which different aspects of parenting stress relate to children’s social-cognitive development and behaviour. Internal consistency alphas have ranged from 0.70 to 0.95. The measure also has high test–retest reliability and validity (Abidin, 1995). Parents completed the rst 60 items with the other questionnaires and they nished the scale during the parent-busy in- teraction.

Child Measures Theory of mind: Theory of mind understanding was assessed with the battery composed by Wellman and Liu (2004). Wellman and Liu developed a scale of theory of mind measures to re ect a broad set of changes in young children’s mental state understanding (desires, emotions, knowledge, and beliefs), rather than only false belief. All of the tasks require children to recognize the subjective nature of mental states. The battery includes seven tasks with easier tasks preceding more dif cult ones. Tasks include assessments of diverse desires, diverse beliefs, knowledge access, contents false belief, explicit false belief, belief emotion, and real-apparent emotion. Dolls and props are used to support the administration of each task (Wellman & Liu, 2004).

For the diverse desires task, children are introduced to Mr Jones who would like a snack. Children then are shown a picture of a carrot and a cookie and are asked which they like best. They are told that Mr Jones likes the snack the child did not choose. For the test question children are asked which snack Mr Jones would choose. The diverse belief task follows the same format, but it focuses on Linda who wants to nd her cat. Children are correct if they are able to indicate that the character ’s desire or belief, respectively, differs from that of the child.

On the knowledge access task, children areasked to guess the contents of a drawer.

Then they are shown the contents. After Polly approaches, children are asked if she knows what is inside of the drawer. Correctresponses require children to understand Polly does not know what is in the drawer because she did not see inside of it.

The contents false belief task involves showing children that a familiar con- tainer holds something unexpected (e.g. a candy box containing a toy pig). For the test question, children are asked what Peter, whom has never seen inside, will think is in the container. The explicit false belief task assesses whether children understand that Scott will look for his mittens where he thinks they are, rather than where they really are.

The last two tasks assess children’s understanding of emotional beliefs. For the belief emotion task children are shown a small box of Cheerios that has rocks inside. They then are told a story about Teddy whose favourite snack is Cheerios.

They are asked how Teddy will feel when he gets the Cheerios box both before and after he sees inside. The real-apparent emotion task involves telling children about Matt who tries to hide his hurt feelings when he is teased on the bus.

Children are asked how Matt really felt and how he tried to look on his face. For the belief emotion and the real-apparent emotion tasks, children have to answer both test questions accurately to be scored as correct. For each of the seven tasks children received one point for a correct response; thus, total scores ranged from 0 to 7 (Wellman & Liu, 2004). N.R. Guajardo et al. 44 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd Emotion understanding: Children’s affective perspective-taking abilities were assessed with the task developed by Cassidy, Parke, Butkovsky, and Braugart (1992). First, the experimenter showed each child four emotion cards (i.e. hap- piness, sadness, anger, and fear), and asked him/her to identify the emotion expressed. If a child did not recognize the emotion the experimenter told him/ her the correct answer and repeated the card sequence until the child responded correctly. All children identi ed the emotions accurately. Next, children were asked to identify what made them, their friend, their mother, and their father experience each emotion. For example, children were shown the happy emotion card and asked: (1) What kinds of things make you feel this way? The same set of questions was asked for each emotion and target person.

Children received a score of 0–4 representing the quality of their response for each of the emotions and target people (self, friend, mother, father). Scores were as follows: 05no response, refusal, don’t know; 15poor response (irrelevant remarks, failure to understand causal nature of question, or cause identi ed that is unlikely to evoke intended emotion); 25adequate response (an appropriate one word or simple clause response); 35good response (relevant sentence-long response or two or more appropriate responses); 45excellent response (elabo- rate response or evidence of insight, including mixed emotions) (Cassidyet al., 1992). Two researchers, blind to demographic and performance variables, coded all responses independently (Cohen’s kappa50.92). Disagreements were re- solved through discussion.

Language Assessment Receptive language was assessed using the Test for the Auditory Comprehension of Language—Third Edition (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1999). The experimenter read a word, a phrase, or a sentence to each child and asked him/her to point to one of the three pictures that best corresponded to the experimenter ’s utterance. Raw scores were used to re ect individual differences in language comprehension rather than age-appropriate performance. Children received a composite score (0–139) depicting receptive language abilities.

Parent–Child Interactions Each parent–child dyad participated in three consecutive 10-min behaviour analogs: free-play, parent-busy, and clean-up tasks. Only data from the free- play and clean-up observations wereused in the data analyses. Parents received instructions from the experimenter prior to each analog. All parent–- child interactions were videotaped using a standard Sony camcorder with remote.

Behaviour analogs: For all interactions, ve large bins of age-appropriate at- tractive toys (e.g. Legos, kitchen set, dolls, and car track) were placed throughout the room. For the rst 10-min segment, parents were instructed to play with their child as they would at home, allowing their child to lead the play activity (Brum eld & Roberts, 1998). After 10 min, parents received instructions for the second observation. For the second 10-min observation (i.e. parent-busy task), parents were told to complete a questionnaire and instruct their child to play independently. To this end, all parents were provided a standardized response that was read by them at the beginning of the observation. For the last ob- servation, parents were instructed to have their child pick up all of the toys and Parenting and Social Cognition45 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd place them in their labelled bins, without actually aiding their child with the clean-up activity.

Coding: The free-play and clean-up tasks were coded according to separate coding schemes adapted from Forehand and McMahon (1981) by Brum eld and Roberts (1998), and originally designed to assess disruptive and de ant child behaviour.

For the free-play analog, the frequencies of six parent verbal response classes were recorded: descriptions, imitations, praise, questions, commands, and criti- cism. Parental descriptions, imitations, and praise statements re ect parental ‘responsiveness’ (see Eyberg, Bessmer, Newcomb, Edwards, & Robinson, 1994) and parental questions, commands, and criticism re ect ‘intrusiveness’ (see McMahon & Forehand, 2004). Parental statements were coded as descriptions if they immediately (within 5 s) followed and directly referred to their child’s play and were not evaluative. Praise was de ned as any positive evaluation of the child or the child’s prior, ongoing, or future behaviour. Physical affection was coded as praise if initiated by parents. Imitation was de ned and coded for all parent behaviour that approximated or extended their child’s verbal or physical behaviour during the interaction. Parental questions were de ned as any inter- rogative statement to which a verbal response from the child was required. In- terrogative ‘tags’ (i.e. ‘huh’) attached to declarative statements were not coded as questions and were either ignored or coded as descriptions (if appropriate).

Criticism was de ned as a negative or disapproving statement about their child or child’s prior, ongoing, or future behaviour. Finally, parental commands were coded when parents verbalized an order, suggestion, rule, or contingency to which the child must respond.

For the clean-up task, parental instructions, child compliance, and child non- compliance were coded. Parental commands were grossly separated into two categories: direct or indirect/vague/repeated (IVR). Direct commands were those that both speci ed and permitted (i.e. allowed suf cient time) a verbal or motoric response from the child. IVR instructions were coded for all commands that speci ed a response that was unable to be completed because of unnecessary repetition by the parent. IVR instructions also were coded in circumstances when either the parent provided a command, but the desired response was unclear or ambiguous, or he/she phrased the command as a question (i.e. ‘Would you clean up for me?’). While it is possible that direct commands may limit the child’s ability to choose whether to complete the assigned task independently, thus demonstrating some internalization of accepted social norms, indirect commands were assumed into the measure of parental ineffectiveness to remain consistent with current standards of clinical practice in this area (see Eyberget al., 1994; McMahon & Forehand, 2004).

Children were coded for compliance if they appropriately responded (verbally or physically) to their parent’s instruction within 5 s of the instruction. If a child failed to respond either verbally or physically to his/her parent’s request within the 5-s interval, the child was coded as being non-compliant. Finally, parents were recorded as having provided praise when they provided any statement connoting approval or acceptance of their child’s behaviour within 5 s of their child’s compliance.

Three indices were created from data obtained during the clean-up task for analyses. An index of the child’s overall compliance or cooperation was created by summing all instances of child compliance and dividing by the sum of all parental instructions, both direct and IVR. An observed measure of parental skill (or effectiveness) was formed by creating a ratio of direct commands to total N.R. Guajardo et al. 46 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd commands (i.e. direct commands/direct1IVR commands). Finally, a likelihood index of parental praise was created by determining the ratio of total contingent praise statements to overall child compliance frequency.

Coding accuracy: Four independent raters unaware of demographic informa- tion and performance scores coded the interactions.Coders were trained to cri- terion together and randomly assigned to an analog such that two people coded each analog. Each coder was randomly assigned to 66% of participants—33% of segments were singly coded, and 33% were coded dually, meaning each coder coded roughly 66% of segments independently. Every 15th segment was coded by both coders together to ensure they continued to follow the same criteria.

Inter-rater agreement was calculated by examining both temporal contiguity and accuracy of each code. Errors were coded if either the order of the code or the agreement of the class of behaviour differed between coders. Inter-rater agree- ment was calculated by summing all errors (both temporal and quality) across each participant family and dividing by total number of agreed upon responses.

This method for establishing inter-rater reliability is well established for the aforementioned parent–child analogs (see Brum eld & Roberts, 1998; Eyberget al., 1994; McMahon & Forehand, 2004). Inter-rater reliability for each analog was:

free-play (r50.95) and clean-up (r50.87). Data from the parent-busy task were used in another study and were not germane to the current investigation. Con- sequently, these data and associated reliability analyses are not reported.

RESULTS Overview of Analyses Analyses are organized into four sections. Preliminary analyses, including descriptive analyses and intercorrelations among measures, are reported rst.

Sections two through four report regression analyses to test the hypotheses. The second section reports analyses of parents’ self-report data, whereas the third section includes analyses of data from observed parent–child interactions. The fourth section explores relationships between theory of mind and emotion understanding and children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviours.

Preliminary Analyses Emotion understanding: Children’s emotion understanding scores for each emotion (happy, sad, angry, and scared) were correlated to examine the appropriateness of a composite score. All correlations were signi cant at po0.001. Internal consistency was high (Cronbach’s alpha50.85); thus, an average emotion understanding score (range from 1 to 4) was calculated for subsequent analyses.

Demographic variables: Relationships among theory of mind, emotion under- standing, age, language, responding parent’s age, responding parent’s education, household income, and number of siblings were explored (see Table 2). As ex- pected, age and language were related to both theory of mind performance and emotion understanding. Thus, they were controlled in subsequent analyses.

Household income also was included in analyses of theory of mind performance given that these two variables were related.

Sex and order: There were no signi cant effects of sex on either theory of mind or emotion understanding scores, though the effect for emotion understanding Parenting and Social Cognition47 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd approached signi cance [t(79)51.92,p50.06], with girls (M52.05; S.D.50.75) scoring slightly better than boys (M51.73; S.D.50.71). The effect of order was not signi cant for theory of mind, but it was for emotion understanding [t(79)52.20,po0.05]. Children who rst completed the emotion understanding task performed better than did those who completed the theory of mind battery rst. Order served as a control variable in subsequent analyses of emotion un- derstanding.

A multivariate analysis of variance was conducted to determine whether par- enting variables differed according to the sex of the child or the sex of the parti- cipating parent. No parenting variables differed according to the sex of the child, p’s40.10. Rates of parental praise and number of commands differed depending upon the sex of the parent. Fathers provided more praise (M57.58; S.D.57.93) and fewer commands (M53.58; S.D.55.16) than did mothers [M53.8, 7.77; S.D.53.19, 6.29, respectively;F’s(1, 45)55.55 and 4.3,p’so0.05]. The difference between fathers’ (M52.80; S.D.50.81) and mothers’ overreactivity (M53.27; S.D.50.96) scores approached signi cance,F(1, 80)53.72,p50.06. Thus, parent sex was entered as a control variables in analyses of these variables.

Relationships among self-reported parenting style,observed parenting style,parenting stress,and child behaviour: As expected, lax parenting was positively associated with parental reports of both internalizing [r(80)50.34,p50.002] and ex- ternalizing [ r(80)50.34,p50.002] symptoms (see Table 3). Only parental reports of child internalizing were related to overreactive parenting [r(80)50.28, p50.01]. Lax parenting was neither associated with dimensions of parental re- sponsiveness (i.e. description, imitation, praise) nor parental intrusiveness (i.e.

questions, commands, criticism;p’s40.10) during the free-play interaction. In contrast, overreactivity was positively associated with total child non-compliance [r(69)50.30,p50.01] observed during the clean-up task.

As expected, parenting stress was related to aspects of both parenting and child behaviour (see Table 3). Speci cally, child-related parenting stress was as- sociated with parental laxness [r(82)50.42,po0.001] as well as child inter- nalizing and externalizing behaviour [r(81)50.47 andr50.51, respectively, p’so0.001]. Parent-related stress was associated with both laxness [r(73)50.55, po0.001] and overreactivity [r(73)50.32,p50.006]. This aspect of parenting stress also predicted child internalizing,r(73)50.40,p50.001, and externalizing, r(73)50.33,p50.004, behaviour. Interestingly, parents’ usage of description during the play interaction was related to both child-related stress,r(47)50.31, p50.02, and parent-related stress,r(47)50.49,p50.002. Table 2. Intercorrelations among theory of mind performance, emotion understanding, child age, receptive language, and demographic variables (N583) Task 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1. Theory of mind — 0.50 0.56 0.61 0.11 0.16 0.32 0.05 2. Emotion understanding — 0.61 0.50 0.04 0.00 0.06 0.04 3. Child age (months) — 0.67 0.00 0.05 0.12 0.16 4. Receptive language — 0.02 0.17 0.19 0.03 5. Parent age — 0.20 0.21 0.23 6. Parent education — 0.36 0.01 7. Household income — 0.23 8. Number of siblings — po0.05; **po0.01; ***po0.001. N.R. Guajardo et al. 48 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd Table 3. Intercorrelations among observed parenting behaviours, self-reported lax parenting, self-reported overreactive parenting, self- reported parenting stress, child externalizing behaviours, and child internalizing behaviour problems (N547) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 1. Lax parenting — 0.53 0.45 0.48 0.15 0.21 0.01 0.08 0.15 0.20 0.18 0.55 0.49 0.04 2. Overreactive parenting — 0.34 0.32 0.08 0.10 0.04 0.06 0.19 0.15 0.17 0.41 0.26 0.22 3. CBCL internalizing — 0.45 0.20 0.18 0.05 0.19 0.00 0.00 0.15 0.54 0.38 0.19 4. CBCL externalizing — 0.08 0.06 0.18 0.05 0.06 0.03 0.11 0.63 0.38 0.26 5. Descriptions (play) — 0.04 0.08 0.09 0.25 0.03 0.26 0.31 0.49 0.31 6. Imitation (play) — 0.37 0.21 0.05 0.14 0.20 0.03 0.01 0.10 7. Praise (play) — 0.12 0.10 0.03 0.02 0.11 0.13 0.11 8. Questions (play) — 0.28 0.17 0.23 0.16 0.03 0.09 9. Commands (play)— 0.02 0.37 0.08 0.07 0.10 10. Criticism (play)— 0.12 0.08 0.00 0.04 11. Parent indirect commands (task)— 0.16 0.08 0.06 12. Child-related stress— 0.73 0.34 13. Parent-related stress— 0.54 14. Life stress— The table re ects results from participants with observational data. *po0.05; **po0.01; ***po0.001. Parenting and Social Cognition49 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd Relationships among observed parenting style,child behaviour,theory of mind,and emotion understanding: There was an inverse relationship between parental com- mands and child compliance,r(47)5 0.30,po0.05 (see Table 4), such that parents who used more commands had children who were less compliant during the clean-up task. Similarly, parental commands and parental criticism were positively related to child non-compliance,r’s(47)50.35,po0.05, and 0.52, po.001, respectively. As would be expected, child non-compliance also was ne- gatively related to child compliance,r(47)5 0.59,po0.001.

Some aspects of parental responsiveness and intrusiveness were related to theory of mind and emotion understanding as well. Speci cally, parental praise predicted theory of mind performance,r(47)5 0.34,po0.05, and parental imi- tation predicted emotion understanding,r(47)5 0.43,po0.01. Finally, child compliance was negatively related to theory of mind,r(47)5 0.37,po0.05, and the relationship with emotion understanding approached signi cance, r(47)5 0.27,p50.08. Surprisingly, all of these relationships were in a negative direction.

Self-reported Parenting Behaviours as Predictors of Children’s Theory of Mind and Emotion Understanding To test relationships between parent-reported parenting behaviours and children’s theory of mind and emotion understanding, hierarchical regressions were conducted for each criterion (i.e. theory of mind performance and emotion understanding). For each analysis, age, language, and sex of child were entered on the rst step. Household income also was included on the rst step for analyses with theory of mind as the dependent variable and task order was Table 4. Intercorrelations among observed parenting behaviours, theory of mind, emotion understanding, and observed child behaviour (N547) 12 34 5 678910 1. Theory of mind— 0.71 0.07 0.18 0.34 0.11 0.02 0.04 0.37 0.21 2. Emotion under- standing— 0.03 0.43 0.27 0.01 0.11 0.08 0.27 1 0.19 3. Parental descriptions— 0.04 0.08 0.09 0.25 1 0.03 0.17 0.05 4. Parental imitations— 0.37 0.21 0.05 0.14 0.05 0.08 5. Parental praise— 0.12 0.10 0.03 0.04 0.07 6. Parental questions— 0.28 1 0.03 0.04 0.32 7. Parental commands— 0.02 0.30 0.35 8. Parental criticism— 0.12 0.52 9. Child compliance— 0.59 10. Child non- compliance— 1po0.10; *po0.05; **pp0.01; ***po0.001. N.R. Guajardo et al. 50 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd included when emotion understanding was the dependent variable. Parent- reported behaviours were entered on the second step in respective analyses.

Laxness and overreactivity: Given the difference between mothers’ and fathers’ overreactivity scores, parent sex was entered as a control variable in these ana- lyses. The rst regression examined theory of mind performance. Each of the control variables, except sex of child and sex of parent, accounted for unique variance in theory of mind performance on the rst step (see Table 5). In the second step, laxness also contributed unique variance to children’s theory of mind performance (beta5 0.208,p50.04), though theR 2change of 0.03 was not signi cant,p50.09. Contrary to the hypotheses, overreactivity did not predict children’s theory of mind performance (beta50.055,p50.59). Neither over- reactive nor lax parenting accounted for unique variance in children’s emotion understanding (R 250.02,p50.36).

Parenting stress: Additional analyses explored the relationship between parent- reported stress (parent related, child related, and life stress) and children’s theory of mind and emotion understanding. Parent-reported life stress emerged as an individual predictor of theory of mind performance (beta5 0.175,p50.05), although the second step of the model was not signi cant (R 250.04,p50.19; see Table 6). Parenting stress did not account for unique variance in emotion un- derstanding (R 250.01,p50.68). Only child age (beta50.397,p50.00) and sex (beta50.227,p50.02) accounted for unique variance in emotion understanding in the nal equation.

Given that both child-related and parent-related parenting stress were related to parental laxness, and laxness predicted children’s theory of mind under- standing, a meditational model was examined. Two regression equations were conducted to examine child-related stress and parent-related stress separately. In each case, the control variables were entered on the rst step and parenting stress and laxness were entered on the second step with theory of mind as the de- pendent variable. In both analyses, laxness remained a signi cant predictor of theory of mind. Thus, no evidence was provided for the idea that parenting (i.e.

laxness) mediates the relationship between parenting stress and theory of mind performance (see Baron & Kenny, 1986). Table 5. Parent-reported lax and overreactive parenting styles predicting children’s theory of mind performance BS.E.BBeta Step 1 Age of child 0.040 0.017 0.263 Receptive language 0.027 0.008 0.384 Sex of child 0.446 0.284 0.133 Household income 0.202 0.073 0.241 Sex of parent 0.049 0.336 0.012 Step 2 Age of child 0.045 0.018 0.287 Receptive language 0.026 0.008 0.372 Sex of child 0.375 0.281 0.112 Household income 0.197 0.073 0.235 Sex of parent 0.005 0.335 0.001 Laxness 0.426 0.198 0.208 Overreactivity 0.097 0.178 0.055 R250.48 ***for Step 1;R 250.51,DR 250.03, ns for Step 2. *po0.05; **po0.01. Parenting and Social Cognition51 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd Observed Parental Responsiveness and Intrusiveness as Predictors of Children’s Theory of Mind and Emotion Understanding Given the small sample size (N547) of observational data, only variables that correlated signi cantly with theory of mind or emotion understanding were included in the following regression analyses. This limited the number of variables entered in each regression. Moreover, power analyses were conducted to examine the appropriateness of regressions with small sample sizes. All regression analyses exceeded the minimal criterion of 0.80 as described by Cohen (1987) for demonstrating adequate levels of power.

As with the analyses of parent-reported data, age, language, and sex of child were entered on the rst step of each analysis. Sex of parent was included as well in the analyses of parental praise. Household income also was included on the rst step for analyses with theory of mind as the dependent variable, whereas task order was included when emotion understanding was the dependent vari- able. Observed parental behaviours were entered on the second step in each case.

The rst analysis examined theory of mind performance. Parental praise ac- counted for marginal unique variance in theory of mind performance beyond the control variables, (R 250.05,p50.06; see Table 7). Unexpectedly, as parental praise increased, children did less well on theory of mind tasks.

A different aspect of parental responsiveness predicted emotion under- standing. Speci cally, parental imitative statements and gestures (i.e. verbal ex- tensions and approximations as well as direct physical imitations of the child’s ongoing play behaviour) predicted children’s emotion understanding (R 250.10, p50.001) such that children with lower emotional competence were more likely to have parents who engaged in greater rates of these imitative activities during the free-play interaction (see Table 8). Parental intrusiveness did not predict theory of mind or emotion understanding.

Child Disruptive Behaviour and Theory of Mind Performance, and Emotion Understanding The following analyses were conducted to examine whether child disruptive behaviour predicted theory of mind and/or emotion understanding. They also Table 6. Parent-reported stress predicting children’s theory of mind performance BS.E.BBeta Step 1 Age of child 0.034 0.018 0.224 1 Receptive language 0.027 0.008 0.410 Sex of child 0.520 0.297 0.157 1 Household income 0.213 0.078 0.249 Step 2 Age of child 0.033 0.018 0.217 1 Receptive language 0.027 0.008 0.411 Sex of child 0.545 0.294 0.165 1 Household income 0.201 0.079 0.234 Parent-related stress 0.005 0.007 0.077 Child-related stress 0.001 0.008 0.008 Life stress 0.013 0.007 0.175 R250.473 ***for Step 1;R 250.510,DR 250.037, ns for Step 2. 1po0.10; *pp0.05; **po0.01; ***po0.001. N.R. Guajardo et al. 52 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd considered whether relationships differed according to child sex. Given the inclusion of an interaction term, all scores were transformed to standardizedz scores for centering purposes.

The rst set of regressions examined whether parent-reported child inter- nalizing and externalizing predicted children’s social-cognitive performance.

Control variables were entered on the rst step in the same manner as with previous regression analyses. Child internalizing and externalizing behaviours, assessed via the CBCL, were entered on the second step. Neither predicted theory of mind performance (R 250.01,p50.43). The interaction terms (Sex by Inter- nalizaing, Sex by Externalizing) were entered on the third step to determine whether potential effects differed for girls and boys. Although the interaction terms did not account for unique variance in theory of mind performance (R 250.03,p50.15), the variance accounted for by the Sex by Externalizing Table 8. Observed parenting behaviour during play predicting children’s average emotion understanding BS.E.Bb Step 1 Age of child 0.032 0.009 0.474 Receptive language 0.011 0.004 0.359 Sex of child 0.112 0.146 0.079 Task order 0.039 0.144 0.028 Step 2 Age of child 0.029 0.008 0.424 Receptive language 0.011 0.003 0.363 Sex of child 0.061 0.130 0.043 Task order 0.056 0.128 0.040 Imitation 0.027 0.008 0.316 R250.594 ***for Step 1;R 250.689,DR 250.10 **for Step 2. **po0.01; ***pp0.001. Table 7. Observed parenting behaviour during play predicting children’s theory of mind performance BS.E.Bb Step 1 Age of child 0.055 0.023 0.351 Receptive language 0.024 0.010 0.323 Sex of child 0.480 0.385 0.145 Household income 0.222 0.122 0.213 Sex of parent 0.568 0.426 0.153 Step 2 Age of child 0.056 0.022 0.354 Receptive language 0.023 0.010 0.313 Sex of child 0.401 0.375 0.121 Household income 0.222 0.118 0.213 1 Sex of parent 0.298 0.434 0.080 Praise 0.074 0.038 0.226 1 R250.487 **for Step 1;R 250.532,DR 250.045 1for Step 2. 1po0.10; *po0.05; **po0.001. Parenting and Social Cognition53 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd variable approached signi cance (B5 0.177,p50.09). The trend suggests an inverse relationship between externalizing behaviour and theory of mind un- derstanding for boys only.

The same analysis was conducted with emotion understanding as the de- pendent variable. The second step of the equation accounted for unique variance in emotion understanding (R 250.05,p50.03; see Table 9). In particular, ex- ternalizing behaviour predicted emotion understanding,B50.237,p50.03. The interaction terms did not account for additional variance,R 250.01,p50.42.

Externalizing scores remained signi cant in the nal equation, such that children with relatively higher externalizing scores performed better on the emotion un- derstanding tasks than did those with lower scores.

Regression analyses also were conducted in a similar manner to examine re- lationships between observed child compliance during the clean-up task and theory of mind and emotion understanding. Neither child compliance nor non- compliance predicted either theory of mind or emotion understanding,p’s40.10. DISCUSSION The current study revealed a number of interesting relationships among parenting, child behaviour, and children’s social-cognitive development. In particular, lax parenting and life stress predicted theory of mind performance, whereas parental imitation during play (an aspect of responsive parenting) was negatively predictive of children’s emotion understanding. Neither externalizing nor internalizing behaviours were predictive of children’s theory of mind Table 9. Child externalizing and internalizing as predictors of children’s average emotion understanding BS.E.Bb Step 1 Age of child 0.387 0.122 0.392 Receptive language 0.247 0.122 0.245 Sex of child 0.174 0.089 0.175 1 Task order 0.152 0.091 0.153 Step 2 Age of child 0.330 0.119 0.333 Receptive language 0.319 0.121 0.316 Sex of child 0.207 0.087 0.209 Task order 0.138 0.088 0.139 Externalizing 0.264 0.102 0.260 Internalizing 0.047 0.098 0.047 Step 3 Age of child 0.325 0.120 0.329 Receptive language 0.323 0.122 0.319 Sex of child 0.199 0.087 0.201 Task order 0.146 0.089 0.147 Externalizing 0.237 0.104 0.233 Internalizing 0.000 0.105 0.000 Sex by Externalizing 0.114 0.106 0.110 Sex by Internalizing 0.138 0.112 0.130 R250.426 ***for Step 1;R 250.480,DR 250.054 *for Step 2;R 250.494,DR 250.013 for Step 3. *po0.05; **pp0.01; ***po0.001. N.R. Guajardo et al. 54 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd development, yet externalizing behaviour was related to emotion understanding.

Finally, the present ndings further support the notion that theory of mind and emotion understanding are related yet distinct domains of social-cognitive development.

Parenting, Theory of Mind, and Emotion Understanding Two interesting relationships were observed between parental behaviour and children’s emotional and cognitive development. Unexpectedly, only lax (i.e.

inconsistent, uninvolved, lackadaisical) parenting predicted poorer performance on theory of mind tasks; however, neither lax nor overreactive parenting predicted children’s emotion understanding. We know that decreased parental engagement and disciplinary inconsistency increase the likelihood that children fail to socialize and are at risk for conduct and mood dif culties (e.g. Loeber, 1990; Patterson, 1982; Patterson, Reid, & Dishon, 1992). Moreover, these children are often seen by parents, teachers, and peers as immature and less popular.

Given the results in the current study, lax/inconsistent/disengaged parenting may affect these changes by signi cantly altering the trajectory of children’s cognitive development, via delayed development.

Parental imitation, an aspect of parental responsivity, negatively predicted children’s emotion understanding. While at rst glance these results contradict current notions of parental responsiveness and earlier ndings with lax parent- ing, there are two compelling reasons to believe that these observations are complementary, not contradictory. First, while it is possible that parental re- sponsiveness, namely imitation, inhibits a child’s emotional development, it is important to understand the cross-sectional and correlational nature of the study.

Speci cally, it is reasonable to surmise that these parental behaviours were used more often with children who required it, scaffolding their cognitive and social development. Rather than parent-driven, the presence of more frequent imitation with children exhibiting more inadequate emotion understanding may result from parental attempts to provide appropriate environmental supports to aid their child’s development. Additional research is needed to explore this ex- planation.

A measure of laxness and overreactivity was chosen for the present study to extend previous work by focusing on parenting approaches associated with child misbehaviour (Arnoldet al., 1993). Indeed, parents who reported behaviour consistent with both lax and overreactive parenting indicated that their children demonstrate externalizing and internalizing behaviours. Consistent with current notions of parenting, self-reported overreactive parenting was associated with repeated, vague, and indirect instructions during subsequent observations. In- effective parental commands also predicted increases in reported child inter- nalizing and externalizing behaviours. These data support previous work suggesting that inconsistent, overly harsh, and lackadaisical parenting practices are associated with increased rates of child behaviour and mood dif culties (Arnoldet al., 1993). These data also provide criterion validity for the parenting measure in the present study.

The assessments of parenting and parent–child behaviours were driven by a behavioural coding system used in clinical settings (Brum eld & Roberts, 1998).

Responsiveness was operationally de ned as describing children’s actions, imi- tating their gestures and statements, and providing praise in a play interaction.

Responsiveness in prior work with theory of mind development included Parenting and Social Cognition55 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd modifying instructions in reaction to child behaviour (Meinset al., 1998) and re ecting on a child’s motives and mental states (Symons & Clark, 2000). These distinctions certainly could account for the differences in ndings. Responsive- ness in the current study was not related to theory of mind development. These data suggest that responsive actions speci c to mental attunement relate to theory of mind development, rather than responsiveness more generally. It would be interesting to examine associations between behavioural and cognitive responsiveness. It seems likely that parents who are responsive in one domain are responsive in another, yet this is an empirical question to be addressed by future research.

Some caution must be taken in interpreting the results from the behavioural observations given that data for only 55% of the participants could be coded. At the least, the ndings are suggestive of interesting relationships among facets of parenting and children’s social-cognitive development.

Parenting Stress, Theory of Mind, and Emotion Understanding The present study indicated that life stress accounted for unique variance in theory of mind performance. These data are consistent with those of Cole and Mitchell (1998) who found an inverse relationship between false belief performance and single-item measures of nancial stress and lone parent stress.

Thus, there is evidence that aspects of parental stress are negatively related to children’s false belief performance. It is possible that parents with relatively low levels of stress are able to engage in parent–child interactions that facilitate children’s theory of mind development; likewise, high stress levels may be associated with family contexts that hinder such understanding. These ndings are inconsistent with those of Symons and Clark (2000) who found that maternal emotional distress at 2 years of age positively predicted children’s false belief performance at 5 years. Taken together, data suggest that aspects of stress related to parenting could have differential relationships with children’s theory of mind understanding.

It was hypothesized that parenting may mediate a relationship between parenting stress and children’s theory of mind and/or emotion understanding.

Though aspects of parenting stress were related to parental laxness, and laxness predicted theory of mind understanding, no support was found for a media- tional relationship. This is further support for the complex nature of family dynamics.

Disruptive Behaviour, Theory of Mind, and Emotion Understanding In contrast to expectations, child externalizing and internalizing behaviours were not related to theory of mind performance, yet externalizing behaviour was related to children’s emotion understanding. Speci cally, children with relatively higher rates of externalizing behaviour demonstrated better understanding of causes of emotion than did children with lower rates of such behaviour. It is possible that children’s externalizing behaviour provides a context in which children learn that others’ emotions differ from their own. These ndings are consistent with previous work demonstrating relationships between parental discipline (Pears & Moses, 2003) and sibling con ict (Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991) and children’s understanding of others’ emotional perspectives. Con ict between family members, either parent–child or child–- N.R. Guajardo et al. 56 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd child, could provide opportunities for children to learn about the relative nature of emotional states.

Some caution needs to be taken in extrapolating from the present data set given the mean scores on the CBCL for both girls and boys. Most children were rated as relatively well adjusted, with very few children obtaining clinically signi cant elevations on any scales. Greater credence to the current results could be provided if this study were replicated with a clinical sample.

Theory of Mind and Emotion Understanding The present study also provides further support for the idea that theory of mind and emotion understanding are somewhat independent areas of social cognition (Cutting & Dunn, 1999; Weimer & Guajardo, 2005). Although related, theory of mind and emotion understanding were differentially associated with observed and reported behaviour for boys and girls. For example, laxness predicted theory of mind performance, whereas parental usage of imitative statements and gestures predicted emotion understanding. Also, life stress was related to children’s theory of mind performance, yet not to their emotion understanding.

These ndings, along with previous work (e.g. Cutting & Dunn, 1999; Weimer & Guajardo, 2005), suggest that these two domains of social cognition overlap, yet they relate differentially to various facets of parent–child interactions. Future research can examine how varying aspects of parenting and contextual factors account for development in these two areas.

Limitations The present study has generated many interesting ndings and considerations for future research. Caution is warranted in a few areas given limitations of the present work, though. The primary limitation concerns the small sample with observational data. Power analyses indicated that the regression analyses examining observational data had ample power to detect effects, yet the sample sizes were less than ideal for regressions. This idea generalizes to the other regressions as well. Given the number of control variables, larger sample sizes could lead to more stable models. Thus, the ndings are suggestive of interesting relationships, but further research with larger samples is needed.

The theoretical model used has emphasized the idea that parental variables (parental stress, parenting) account for individual differences in children’s social- cognitive development. Multiple regressions strengthen conclusions about di- rection of effect, yet they remain correlational analyses: true direction of effects cannot be determined. It is plausible that children’s behaviour or social-cognitive development indeed impacts parents’ stress or parenting styles. Most likely, there are dynamic relationships among these variables. Future work should consider this possibility.

CONCLUSIONS Using both observational and self-report methods, the present study suggests complex relationships among parenting, parenting stress, child behaviour, and children’s social-cognitive performance. Lax parenting approaches and life stress both predicted children’s theory of mind performance, whereas parents’ usage of Parenting and Social Cognition57 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd imitative gestures and vocalizations was related to their emotion understanding.

Children’s own behaviour also related to their social-cognitive performance such that children’s externalizing behaviours were related to their understanding of the causes of emotions. Thus, there were associations between ineffective parenting approaches and child misbehaviour, and between aspects of child misbehaviour and early theory of mind and emotion understanding. The present study provides additional evidence that the social context of parents and their children accounts for aspects of early social-cognitive development.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This project was conducted at Idaho State University with support from a Faculty Research Grant awarded to the rst author. We are grateful to Stefane Drayton, Katie Elkington, Lyn Gibson, and Stacey Hoem for their assistance with data collection and/or coding. We appreciate the cooperation of the children and parents who participated in the study. We also would like to thank Drs Kelly Cartwright, Jeffrey Gibbons, Sherman Lee, and Timothy Marshall for their thoughtful suggestions regarding this manuscript.

APPENDIX Examples of coding categories for the free-play observation:

Responsiveness:Descriptions: Parent: ‘You have the blue block’.Praise (P): Parent: ‘You draw so well’.Imitations (I): Child: ‘I’ve got the block now’.

Parent: ‘Oh, you have it now’.

Intrusiveness:

Questions: Parent: ‘Do you need some help?’Commands: Parent: ‘Play with the blocks’.Criticism: Parent: ‘That’s not good at all’.

REFERENCES Abidin, R. R . (1995).Parenting stress index (PSI) manual(3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Achenbach, T. M . (1991).Manual for the child behavior checklist and 1991 pro le. Burlington, VT: Department of psychiatry, University of Vermont.

Arnold, D. S., O’Leary, S. G., Wolff, L. S., & Acker, M. M. (1993). The parenting scale: A measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations.Psychological Assessment,5, 137–144.

Arranz, E., Artamendi, J., Olabarrieta, F., & Martin, J. (2002). Family context and theory of mind development.Early Child Development and Care,172, 9–22.

Astington, J. (1996). What is theoretical about the child’s theory of mind?: A Vygotskian view of its development. In P. Carruthers, & P. K. Smith (Eds.),Theories of theory of mind (pp. 184–199). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Badenes, L. V., Estevan, R. A. C., & Bacete, F. J. G. (2000). Theory of mind and peer rejection.Social Development,9, 271–283.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,51, 1173–1182. N.R. Guajardo et al. 58 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd Bjorkqvist, K., Osterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1999). Social intelligence minus empathy5aggression?Aggression and Violent Behavior,5, 191–200.

Bowlby, J. (1982).Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). Attachment(2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979, 2006).The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Brown, J. R., Donelan-McCall, N., & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about mental states? The signi cance of children’s conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers.Child Development,67, 836–849.

Brum eld, B. D., & Roberts, M. W. (1998). A comparison of two measurements of child compliance with normal preschool children.Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,27, 109–116.

Capage, L., & Watson, A. C. (2001). Individual differences in theory of mind, aggressive behavior, and social skills in young children.Early Education and Development,12, 614–628.

Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999).Test for auditory comprehension of language(3rd ed.). Austin, TX:

Pro-ed.

Cassidy, J., Parke, R. D., Butkovsky, L., & Braugart, J. M. (1992). Family–peer connections:

The roles of emotional expressiveness within the family and children’s understanding of emotions.Child Development,63, 605–618.

Cassidy, K. W., Werner, R. S., Rourke, M., Zubernis, L. S., & Balaraman, G. (2003). The relationship between psychological understanding and positive social behaviors.Social Development,12, 198–221.

Cohen, J. (1987).Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences(Rev. ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cole, K., & Mitchell, P. (1998). Family background in relation to deceptive ability and understanding of mind.Social Development, 7, 182–197.

Cutting, A., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations.Child Development, 70, 853–863.

Denham, S. A., & Couchoud, E. A. (1990). Young preschoolers’ understanding of emotions.

Child Study Journal, 20, 171–192.

Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991) Family talk about emotions, and children’s later understanding of others’ emotions.Developmental Psychology, 27, 448–455.

Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents.Child Development, 62, 1352–1366.

Entwisle, D. R., & Astone, N. M. (1994). Some practical guidelines for measuring youth’s race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status.Child Development, 65, 1521–1540.

Eyberg, S., Bessmer, J., Newcomb, K., Edwards, D., & Robinson, E. (1994).Dyadic parent– child interaction coding system II: A manual.San Rafael, CA: Select Press.

Fonagy, P., Redfern, S., & Charman, T. (1997). The relationship between belief–desire reasoning and a projective measure of attachment security (SAT).British Journal of Developmental Psychology,15, 51–61.

Forehand, R . L ., & McMahon, R . J . (1981).Helping the non-compliant child: A clinicians guide to parent training. New York: The Guilford Press.

Happe, F., & Frith, U. (1996). Theory of mind and social impairment in children with conduct disorder.British Journal of Developmental Psychology,14 , 385–398.

Hembree-Kigin, T . L ., & McNeil, C . B . (1995).Parent– child interaction therapy. New York:

Plenum Press.

Hughes, C., Deater-Deckard, K., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). ‘‘Speak roughly to your little boy’’? Sex differences in the relations between parenting and preschoolers’ under- standing of mind.Social Development,8, 143–160.

Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat?: Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in ‘‘hard-to-manage’’ preschoolers.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,7, 981–994.

Humfress, H., O’Connor, T. G., Slaughter, J., Target, M., & Fonagy, P. (2002). General and relationship-speci c models of social cognition: Explaining the overlap and discrepan- cies.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,43, 873–883.

Laible, D., & Thompson, R. A. (1998). Attachment and emotional understanding in preschool children.Developmental Psychology,34, 1038–1045. Parenting and Social Cognition59 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd Lalonde, C. E., & Chandler, M. J. (1995). False belief understanding goes to school: On the social–emotional consequences on coming early or late to a rst theory of mind.

Cognition and Emotion,9, 167–186.

Loeber, R. (1990). Development and risk factors of juvenile antisocial behavior and delinquency.Clinical Psychology Review,10, 1–41.

McLoyd, V. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black families and children:

Psychological distress, parenting and socioemotional development.Child Development, 61, 311–346.

McMahon, R . J . & Forehand, R. L . (2004).Helping the non-compliant child: A clinicians guide to parent training(2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

Meins, E. (1997). Security of attachment and maternal tutoring strategies: Interaction within the zone of proximal development.British Journal of Developmental Psychology,15, 129–144.

Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (1999). Linguistic acquisitional style and mentalising development: The role of maternal mind-mindedness.Cognitive Development,14, 363–380.

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Russel, J., & Clark-Carter, D. (1998). Security of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and mentalising abilities: A longitudinal study.Social Development, 7, 1–24.

Minuchin, S., & Fishman, H. C. (1981).Family therapy techniques.London: Harvard University Press.

Patterson, G. R. (1982).Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia Press.

Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishon, T. J. (1992).Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia.

Pears, K. C., & Moses, L. J. (2003). Demographics, parenting, and theory of mind in preschool children.Social Development,12, 1–20.

Raver, C. C., Blackburn, E. K., Bancroft, M., & Torp, N. (1999). Relations between effective emotional self-regulation, attentional control, and low-income preschoolers’ social competence with peers.Early Education and Development,10, 332–350.

Reitman, D., Currier, R. O., & Hupp, S. D. (2001). Psychometric characteristics of the parenting scale in a head start population.Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,30, 514–524.

Ruffman, T., Perner, J., & Parkin, L. (1999). How parenting style affects false belief understanding.Social Development,8, 395–411.

Sutton, J., Reeves, M., & Keogh, E. (2000). Disruptive behavior, avoidance of responsibility and theory of mind.British Journal of Developmental Psychology,18, 1–11.

Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Bullying and ‘theory of mind’: A critique of the ‘social skills de cit’ view of anti-social behaviour.Social Development,8, 117–127.

Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). Social cognition and bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation?British Journal of Developmental Psychology,17, 435–450.

Symons, D. K., & Clark, S. E. (2000). A longitudinal study of mother–child relationships and theory of mind in the preschool period.Social Development,9, 3–23.

Vinden, P. G. (2001). Parenting attitudes and children’s understanding of mind: A comparison of Korean American and Anglo-American families.Cognitive Development, 16, 793–809.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman (Eds.),Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Watson, A. C., Nixon, C. L., Wilson, A., & Capage, L. (1999). Social interaction skills and theory of mind in young children.Developmental Psychology,35, 386–391.

Webster-Stratton, C. (2000).The incredible years training series bulletin. Washington, DC:

Of ce of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, US Department of Justice.

Weimer, A. A., & Guajardo, N. R. (2005). False belief, emotion understanding, and social skills among head start and non-head start children.Early Education and Development,16, 341–366.

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief.Child Development,72, 655–684.

Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks.Child Development,75, 523–541. N.R. Guajardo et al. 60 Copyrightr2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Inf. Child. Dev.18: 37 – 60 (2009) DOI: 10.1002/icd