1. find your answers o the research topic o research problem o research question 2. What is the research aim? Hypothesis? if it is a hypothesis, if it is non-directional or directional 3

Nonmainstream Dialect Use and Specific Language Impairment

Janna B. Oetting and Janet L. McDonald

Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge

Abstract Most work looking at specific language impairment (SLI) has been done in the context of

mainstream dialects. This paper extends the study of SLI to two nonmainstream dialects: a rural

version of Southern African American English (SAAE) and a rural version of Southern White

English (SWE). Data were language samples from 93 4- to 6-year-olds who lived in southeastern

Louisiana Forty were classified as speakers of SAAE, and 53 were classified as speakers of SWE.

A third were previously diagnosed σs SLI; the others served as either age-matched (6N) or

language-matched (4N) controls.

The two dialects differed in frequency of usage on 14 of the 35 coded morphosyntactic surface

patterns; speakers of these dialects could be successfully discriminated (94%) from each other in a

discriminant analysis using just four of these patterns. Across dialects, four patterns resulted in

main effects that were related to diagnostic condition (SLI vs. 6N), and a slightly different set of

four patterns showed effects that were related to developmental processes (4N vs. 6N). More

interestingly, the surface characteristics of SLI were found to manifest in the two dialects in

different ways. A discriminant function based solely on SAAE speakers tended to misclassify

SWE children with SLI as having normal language, and a discriminant function based on SWE

speakers tended to misclassify SAAE unaffected children as SLI. Patterns within the SLI profile

that cut across the two dialects included difficulties with tense marking and question formation.

The results provide important direction for future studies and argue for the inclusion of contrastive

as well as noncontrastive features of dialects within SLI research.

Keywords dialect; specific language impairment; morphosyntax

In a recent publication, Tager-Flusberg and Cooper (1999) summarize the comments of

participants from an NIH-sponsored workshop that focused on the study of specific

language impairment (SLI). The participants included experts in the fields of SLI and other

developmental disorders such as autism, learning disabilities, and dyslexia. As part of the

report, the authors highlight important study topics to help guide future research. One of the

recommendations listed in the report is the development of constructs that are important for

defining SLI in individuals who come from many language, cultural, and dialect

backgrounds.

The goal of the current work is to begin, at an exploratory level, to extend the study of SLI

to two nonmainstream dialects of English. Although the grammatical profile of SLI has been

explored in a wide range of languages, including Dutch, English, French, German, Greek,

Hebrew, Hungarian, Italian, Japanese, Spanish, Swedish, and even Inukitut (an Eskimo-

© 2001. All rights reserved

Contact author: Janna B. Oetting, PhD, Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, 163 M&DA Bldg., Louisiana State

University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803. [email protected]. NIH Public Access

Author Manuscript

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

Published in final edited form as:

J Speech Lang Hear Res . 2001 February ; 44(1): 207–223.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Aleut language) (Leonard, 1998), the study of SLI within nonmainstream varieties of these

languages has been nonexistent. A primary reason for this has been limited knowledge on

the part of researchers about the developmental trajectories of nonmainstream dialects. In

fact, our limited understanding of dialect acquisition has led many to argue that

nonmainstream patterns should be ignored or excluded when diagnosing a child as language

impaired (Battle, 1996; Leonard & Weiss, 1983; McGregor, Williams, Hearst, & Johnson,

1997; Seymour, 1986; Seymour, Bland-Stewart, & Green, 1998; Stockman, 1996). The

rationale for excluding dialectal forms within analyses is to guard against viewing a

language difference (i.e., dialect difference) as a language impairment. As noted by

Seymour et al. (1998), evaluation of nonmainstream patterns establishes a “diagnostic

conundrum” because some patterns of nonmainstream dialect, on the surface, can look very

similar to those that characterize a language impairment.

For theoretical studies of SLI, however, a major limitation of excluding nonmainstream

features within one’s analysis is that decisions about what may or may not be important for

characterizing the impairment are made a priori. Moreover, after dialectal patterns are

excluded, the types of structures a researcher is left to examine are not always useful for

testing existing models of impairment. Data from Seymour et al. (1998) illustrate this point.

Within their study, they use the terms

contrastive and noncontrastive to differentiate

grammatical surface patterns that are unique to a particular dialect (i.e., contrastive) from

those that surface in both nonmainstream and standard English varieties (i.e.,

noncontrastive). Of the 11 structures that they considered to be noncontrastive, only 5 (i.e.,

complex sentences, conjunctions, locatives, modals, and verb particles) can be readily

applied to children who speak other nonmainstream English dialects, such as those spoken

in the rural South.

Moreover, only three of the noncontrastive patterns examined by Seymour et al. showed

significant group differences (i.e., use of prepositions, subjective marking of pronouns, and

demonstrative marking of pronouns). On these three, average use of the standard English

surface forms by the children with a language impairment also was quite high, at or above

89%. Thus, even if one were to develop a theoretical account for the co-existence of these

particular group differences, one would have to question their clinical relevance. To the

authors’ credit, they do acknowledge this point. Nevertheless, they maintain that until more

is known about the “deficit/difference” distinction within nonmainstream dialects, clinical

efforts should focus on the noncontrastive features of language development.

Unlike the clinical work cited above, children’s use of nonmainstream surface patterns is the

focal point of the current study. The primary goal of the project is to determine if the

morphosyntactic limitations of children with SLI can be studied within the context of

nonmainstream dialect use. Although this research objective resonates well with the

recommendations put forth by the participants of the NIH workshop, the impetus driving the

work has been our observations of nonmainstream dialect use among children who live in

one rural area of southeastern Louisiana. In particular, we have been struck by differences in

the children’s use of nonmainstream surface patterns that seem dependent upon both the

child’s dialect type and language ability.

As a preliminary study, we examined the grammatical features of SLI using language

samples from 31 children whom we perceived as using a variety of Southern White English

(SWE) (Oetting, Cantrell, & Horohov, 1999). Nine of the children were classified as

language impaired; the others served as either age-matched or language-matched controls.

Samples were elicited by an adult examiner within a small room at each child’s school.

Although no group differences were evident when either the types of nonmainstream

features spoken or the percentage of utterances with one or more nonmainstream dialect

Oetting and McDonald Page 2

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript pattern was evaluated, differences did emerge in the children’s use of some key grammatical

structures. These included regular third-person marking, contractible copula

be, auxiliary be,

and third-person marking of

do. In all cases, the children with SLI were found to be less

productive (i.e., either produced fewer marked forms or fewer obligatory contexts) with

these structures than their age- and language-matched controls.

In the current work, we continue this line of inquiry by expanding the data set to include

samples from children perceived to speak a rural variety of Southern African American

English (SAAE) and additional samples from children perceived to speak a variety of SWE.

Before outlining the research questions that guided the work, we review characteristics of

SAAE and SWE.

Characterstics of SAAE and SWE Nonmainstream dialects in the United States have been studied for at least five decades.

Like most areas of scientific inquiry, a number of issues remain unresolved and, thus,

continue to fuel rigorous debate and on-going inquiry. Opinion differs as to the origin,

current relationship, and direction of language change that is occurring within, and across

African American and White English varieties (for review see Baugh, 1983; Butters, 1989;

Montgomery & Bailey, 1986; Mufwene et al., 1998; Spears, 1992). Consensus has been

difficult because historical records are incomplete or lacking, and a number of within-

subject and between-subject factors can influence findings (Bailey & Bernstein, 1990;

Rickford, Ball, Blake, Jackson, & Martin, 1991; Wolfram, 1990). Within-subject variables

include age-graded phenomena, developmental processes, context effects, and code

switching/style shifting. Between-subject variables include race, age, sex, social class,

region, community, length of residency, and language contact. For the work here, a further

complication is the finding that much of the research on nonmainstream varieties of English

has been completed in urban settings located in the North, and study participants frequently

have been adolescents or adults who produce moderate to extreme versions of the

vernacular. Although these findings are interesting, they do not necessarily reflect the

patterns of all speakers in all discourse situations (Bailey & Bernstein, 1990).

Studies of SAAE and SWE do exist, however. Within these studies, participants are

typically selected from speech communities that are known to present the nonmainstream

dialect(s) of interest. Thus, the use of the terms SAAE and SWE are shorthand for

describing the patterns of specific linguistic groups—not all people who live in the South

nor all people of a particular race. Within comparative studies of these two dialects, effort

also is made to choose participants who present similar sociodemographic profiles. In some

studies, a full stratified sampling method has been employed; in others, participants have

been selected using a particular set of sociodemographic criteria, such as place of residence,

school enrollment, or SES (see edited works in Bernstein, Nunnally, & Sabino, 1997 and

Montgomery & Bailey, 1986).

The results of these studies and others indicate that SAAE and SWE are more similar, at

least on the surface, than northern dialect varieties. Nevertheless, both qualitative and

quantitative differences between the two have been documented. Qualitative differences

between SAAE and SWE primarily involve tense and aspect (see Bailey & Maynor, 1985,

1987; Green, 1993; Mufwene et al., 1998; Rickford, 1985). These include, but are not

limited to, grammatical constructions involving habitual or durative

be (often referred to as

be2 ), done, be done , and stressed been (referred to by some as BIN ). Many of these

structures are considered camouflaged forms because their surface forms (e.g., be, done)

appear in standard English and other White English varieties, but in African American

English varieties they express a wider range of meanings within the grammar. For example,

Oetting and McDonald Page 3

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Labov (1998) cites the following utterance—“So he went to where she was…and got the

nerve to lie to me…talking ’bout he

done went to work”—from Baugh (1983) to illustrate

the sense of moral indignation that the use of

done can carry in African American English.

Examination of the co-occurrence restrictions (i.e., what types of auxiliaries, verbs, and

adverbs can or cannot co-occur with these structures) and manipulation of these forms

within negative and past-tense constructions, tag questions, and yes/no questions also

indicate that their underlying grammatical structure in African American English varieties is

qualitatively different from other White English varieties.

Arguably, the greatest difference between SAAE and SWE is the frequency with which

different nonmainstream patterns are produced. This claim is based on the finding that many

of the same nonmainstream patterns surface in both dialects, but speakers of SAAE have

been found to produce these forms more frequently than speakers of SWE (Feagin, 1997;

Montgomery & Bailey, 1986; Mufwene et al., 1998; Wolfram & Schilling-Estes, 1998).

Zero-marking of verbal /s/ with third-person singular subjects (e.g., he walk) is commonly

used to illustrate this point. As discussed by Wolfram and Schilling-Estes (1998), this

particular nonmainstream pattern is present in both dialects, but within SAAE and other

African American English varieties, zero-marking can occur 85% of the time or more. In

SWE varieties, zero-marking can occur less then 5% of the time.

Traditionally, quantitative differences have been viewed as indicating similarities in the

deep structure of these dialects; however, there now is evidence that some quantitative

differences may reflect qualitative differences in rule construction as well. Work by Myhill

(1988) on postvocalic

is useful for illustrating this point. He examined

production in

two groups of speakers of African American English: those with frequent contact with

mainstream culture and those without. As expected, quantitative differences between the two

groups were observed, with the former producing

more frequently than the latter.

Important for the work here is the finding that the two groups’ use of

also was influenced

by the phonological characteristics of the preceding and following context in slightly

different ways. These differences can be considered qualitative in nature because they reflect

differences in the ways in which

production is constrained within the two linguistic

systems.

For researchers interested in extending the study of childhood SLI to nonmainstream

dialects, the status of dialectology research, the overlapping characteristics of SAAE and

SWE, and the overlapping characteristics of SLI and nonmainstream dialects may be viewed

as daunting. Clearly, a study of children’s use of nonmainstream patterns, especially if it

seeks only to identify surface differences between dialects and child language profiles,

should be viewed as a first step toward more empirical studies on the issue. With the above

discussion and qualifying statements in mind, we asked the following questions: (a) Can

children’s use of nonmainstream surface patterns be used to differentiate SWE from SAAE?

(b) Can effects of SLI be observed within the context of these nonmainstream patterns? and

(c) If effects of SLI are observed, do these effects differ as a function of the type of dialect

spoken?

Method

Participants The data were spontaneous language samples from 93 children who had previously

participated in one of two studies (Oetting, 1999; Oetting & Horohov, 1997). Across studies,

there were 40 children who were classified as speakers of SAAE and 53 who were classified

as speakers of SWE; 60% of the children were male. For the work here, the children were

classified as speakers of either SAAE or SWE using the extrinsic criterion of race. Grouping

Oetting and McDonald Page 4

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript the children in this way allows for linguistic description of the participants, and the analyses

to be conducted provide a direct test of the criterion that led to the grouping.

As discussed in the original papers, all of the children attended public schools, child

development centers, or Head Starts that were located in a rural area in southeastern

Louisiana. This area is situated on the Mississippi River and maintains a large port industry

involving natural and synthetic products. The social strata of the children’s families ranged

from skilled craft, clerical, and sales groups to small business, minor professional, and

technical groups. Estimates of the children’s social strata were based on the parent(s)’

highest level of education completed and occupation (Hollingshead, 1975). Data for this

index were collected through a voluntary questionnaire that was returned by 19 (48%) of the

SAAE and 41 (77%) of she SWE speakers. Responses from the questionnaires also

indicated that all of the children, who had returned the form had lived in southeastern

Louisiana since birth.

Thirty-one of the children were classified as SLI; 31 were identified as normally developing,

age-matched 6-year-olds (6N); and 31 were identified as normally developing, language-

matched 4-year-olds (4N). Classification of a child’s language status involved a number of

steps to guard against inaccurate diagnosis. These included soliciting children during the

second half of the school year so that any misdiagnosis that may have occurred at the

beginning of the year could be corrected, conducting teacher and SLP interviews to

document impressions of language status that were based on comparisons with classroom

peers, completing a battery of language tests, and collecting a language sample during an

informal play session. Although details related to participant selection are described

elsewhere, important demographic information and testing data are broken down by dialect

and diagnostic category in Table 1.

The highest level of education completed by the children’s mothers was available for the 60

participants who returned the parent questionnaire. Following Hollingshead (1975), a score

of 3 reflected completion of 8th grade, a 4 reflected completion of 12th, and a 5 reflected

completion of two years of college or additional vocational training. Although the mode

level of education was high school (4), a two-way analysis of variance showed a significant

interaction between dialect and group on this variable [

F(2, 59) = 3.41, p = .04]. Tukey post

hoc tests indicated that the interaction was related to higher scores for the SWE speakers

than for the SAAE speakers for the 4N group.

Differences related to diagnostic category but not dialect were significant for the Columbia

Mental Maturity Scale (CMMS; Burgmeister, Blum, & Lorge, 1972) [

F(2, 90) = 6.90, p < .

005], the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1981) [

F(2,

92) = 83.90,

p < .001], the syntactic quotient score of the Test of Language Development–

Primary (TOLD-P; Newcomer & Hammill, 1988), [

F(2, 80) = 70.69, p < .001], and mean

length of utterance (MLU) regardless of whether MLU was calculated with words [

F(2, 92)

= 12.53,

p < .001] or with morphemes [ F(2, 92) = 13.56, p < .001]. Tukey tests indicated

that for the CMMS, PPVT-R, and TOLD-P, scores of the SLI group were lower than those

of the 6N and 4N group. For MLU-words and MLU-morphemes, scores of the SLI and 4N

groups were lower than those of the 6N group.

Language Sample Elicitation Language samples were elicited by having an examiner and child play together in a quiet

room within each child’s school. Samples were collected on the third day of meeting with

each child; standardized testing was completed during the first two sessions. Toys included a

gas station, picnic/park set, baby dolls, food set, Legos, Mardi Gras beads, and three pictures

from the Apricot I Picture Series (Arwood, 1985). All six examiners were White, and

Oetting and McDonald Page 5

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript although three were native to Louisiana and spoke a dialect that contained some Southern

characteristics, the language of all six during the study contained primarily standard English

grammatical forms. Visual inspection of the raw data did not reveal systematic patterning of

dialect use by the children that was dependent upon the native versus non-native resident

status of the examiners.1

A two-way analysis of variance was completed to determine whether the length of the

samples varied by dialect or diagnostic category. The average number of complete and

intelligible utterances per sample was the dependent measure. A main effect for dialect was

observed [

F(l, 92) = 6.20, p < .05], with samples elicited from the SAAE speakers shorter

than those elicited from the SWE speakers. It is important to note, however, that samples

from Oetting and Horohov (1997) were longer than those from Oetting (1999), and the

number of children who spoke each dialect was not consistent across studies. In the 1997

study, 94% of the children spoke SWE as compared to 35% in the 1999 study. Biases related

to dialect are not evident when the samples from the individual studies are examined

separately. For example, the average length of the samples from the SAAE speakers who

participated in Oetting (1999) was 202 (

SD = 65) and for the SWE speakers, it was 208 ( SD

= 59), p > .05. Therefore, the sampling bias observed here seems more a product of

differences in the time spent on language-sample collection during the original studies than

on the variable of dialect.2

Language Sample Transcription and Coding At the time of the original studies (1994–1995 and 1996–1997), the samples were

transcribed and frequently discussed nonmainstream patterns were coded. After extensive

literature review and consultation with native speakers, all tapes were replayed and

utterances were further coded for infrequent nonmainstream patterns (1998–1999). For both

phases, transcription and morphological coding followed the guidelines outlined by Miller

and Chapman (1992). Word processing Find/Replace commands and Systematic Analysis of

Language Transcripts software (SALT; Miller & Chapman, 1992) were used to facilitate and

check coding. Frequency counts of each dialect pattern were based on SALT printouts. At

least two examiners counted the patterns from the SALT printouts independently;

disagreements between examiners were resolved through consensus.

Thirty-five different types of nonmainstream patterns were identified in the 20,171

utterances that were transcribed as complete and intelligible. A pattern was considered

nonmainstream if it had been previously reported as a SWE or SAAE pattern in the literature

and if our native consultants felt that the pattern was characteristic of the dialects they heard

children and adults producing within this community.3

Although the criteria used to identify each nonmainstream pattern are listed in the Appendix,

at least three issues related to coding need to be highlighted. First, lists of dialect patterns

vary greatly across studies and are dependent upon the interests of the researcher(s) and the

characteristics of the participants under study. The patterns examined here reflect our

1 Although there were no obvious effects of examiner characteristics on outcomes, that possibility cannot be fully ruled out. To

adequately study the role of the examiner on dialect use, one would need equal numbers of African American and White examiners,

equal numbers of male and female examiners, and equal numbers of examiners whose use of nonmainstream patterns matched and did

not match those of the participants See Lucas and Borders (1994) for an ethnographic study of children’s use of nonmainstream

patterns across different partners and discourse contexts.

2 Differences across studies also played some role in the unequal numbers of parent questionnaires that were returned by the SAAE

and SWE speakers In Oetting (1999), when dialect use was more evenly distributed across the participants, the numbers of

questionnaires returned by the SAAE and SWE speakers were 18 and 19, respectively.

3 Patterns were considered nonmainstream regardless of whether they overlapped with patterns known to be characteristic of SLI and\

/

or with patterns known to be produced by younger, normally developing children.

Oetting and McDonald Page 6

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript interests in morphosyntax. Second, for some morphosyntactic categories, more than one

type of surface pattern was identified. For example, five different patterns (i.e., zero regular,

zero irregular, over-regularization, past as participle,

had + past) were coded for past tense,

and three (i.e., zero regular, zero irregular, subject-verb agreement with

don’t ) were coded

for third-person marking. Arguably some of these patterns or even patterns across

grammatical categories could be combined. Nevertheless as an exploratory study, these

individual patterns were counted separately to examine the independent variables of interest.

Finally, some scholars may disagree with particular coding decisions made for individual

patterns. For example, children’s variable use of historical present within narrative contexts

made it difficult to determine whether an unmarked form (e.g., she kick him) should be

coded as zero-marked for present third (e.g., she kicks him) or past tense (e.g., she kicked

Mm). As discussed by Wolfram (1990), nonmainstream dialects allow tense marking to

alternate not only across utterances but within utterances as well. Our decision here was to

accept all verbs that were marked for third person (e.g., he comes) as standard English uses,

even though some were clearly produced as overt uses of historical present. Also, unmarked

forms of the verb

say (e.g., she say stop that) were coded as involving zero-marking of third-

person present rather than past because (a) this verb is frequently produced in historical

present contexts, and (b) when produced in these contexts, this verb is typically unmarked

(Myhill & Harris, 1986). All other unmarked verbs were coded as present or past depending

upon the context and meaning of the utterance.

In Table 2, frequency counts of each nonmainstream pattern are presented. Although these

data are interesting, they should be viewed as crude indices of use because the number of

utterances in each sample was not controlled. To control for sample size differences, all

analyses were completed by dividing the total number of each nonmainstream pattern by the

total number of complete and intelligible utterances in each sample. In traditional studies of

SLI, percent of use often is calculated by dividing the total number of produced forms by the

total number of obligatory contexts for each structure. With some nonmainstream patterns,

however, the concept of an obligatory context is untenable. For example, there are no known

obligatory contexts for patterns that mark aspectual distinctions (e.g.,

be2 , done + verb)

(Seymour et al., 1998). Also, for past tense, third person, and others, more than one type of

nonmainstream surface pattern can be produced. Variable use of these different patterns

makes it difficult, if not impossible, to determine the exact number of obligatory contexts for

each. Finally, as described in the Appendix, some nonmainstream patterns (e.g., zero

be,

multiple negation, and undifferentiated pronoun) are much more likely to occur in certain

linguistic contexts than in others. Without a comprehensive study of how different contexts

influence nonmainstream pattern use, explicating the number of obligatory contexts for each

pattern remains elusive. Using the total number of utterances as the denominator when

calculating the rate at which each nonmainstream pattern occurred allowed us to (a) examine

each nonmainstream pattern in the same way, (b) avoid making a priori coding decisions

about obligatory contexts, and (c) control for sample size differences.

Reliability Transcription agreement was calculated at the utterance level because an error on a word,

morpheme, utterance boundary, or dialect code can affect the reliability of an entire

utterance. Also, reliability was calculated on the child utterances only, even though the adult

utterances were typed and checked. Nine (10%) of the samples were independently

transcribed and coded by a research assistant at the time of data collection. Across studies

and groups, intertranscriber agreement was 95% (2498 utterances in agreement/2641 total

child utterances). An additional 13 (14%) of the samples were checked by the principal

investigator after all nonmainstream patterns were coded; half of the samples were from

SAAE speakers, and a third were from each of the three child groups. Checking included

Oetting and McDonald Page 7

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript listening to the audiotapes while reading the transcripts and then proofing the coding.

Agreement was 93% (2602 utterances in agreement/2793 total child utterances; SAAE =

93.5%, SWE = 93.2%; SLI = 93.7%, 6N = 92.1%, 4N = 94.1%). An additional six (6%) of

the audiotapes/samples were independently checked by a teacher from one of the targeted

Head Start programs. This teacher was chosen because she had lived in the area her entire

life, her native dialect was SAAE, and as a bus driver for Head Start and mother of two she

interacted with children of this age range outside of the formal school setting. She disagreed

with the transcription of 19 (<1%) words from a total of 7159 total words checked.

Results Given that dialect use was not part of the participant selection process, several preliminary

steps were completed to confirm the children’s dialect status.4 First, the number of different

nonmainstream pattern types was counted for each child; all children were found to produce

at least 5 of the 35 different nonmainstream patterns. Next, the total number of

nonmainstream patterns produced by each child was divided by the total number of

complete and intelligible utterances in each sample (see Table 3). As can be seen, all of the

children produced a rate of nonmainstream dialect use that was .03 or greater. Both the type-

and token-based counts of pattern use verified to us that all of the children were

nonmainstream speakers.

General ranges of the rate at which the nonmainstream patterns were produced by the

children are also listed in Table 3. Note that there is very little overlap in the rates at which

the SAAE speakers produce the nonmainstream patterns as compared to those of the SWE

speakers, especially when one considers the three diagnostic categories (SLI, 6N, 4N)

separately. A difference in pattern frequency between the SAAE and SWE speakers is

consistent with our literature review, even though most of the previous work has been

completed with adults. However, as noted at the bottom of this table, 4 children did fall

outside the usage rates for their same-dialect child group. These particular rates of use raise

the possibility that these children are classified in the wrong dialect group. In order to check

this, a listener judgment task was preformed on the audiotaped samples of these four

children. When listeners blind to the identity of these children listened to 10 min of each

sample, the 3 African American children were identified as speakers of SAAE and the 1

White child was identified as a speaker of SWE.5 These findings, taken together, confirmed

our initial impressions that the data set contained two distinct dialects.

The data were then subjected to two sets of analyses. Analyses of variance were first

completed to examine whether nonmainstream pattern use varied as a function of dialect

type and/or child group. A series of discriminant analyses were then run to see how

discriminable the dialects and child groups were from each other and to ascertain the

minimal set of patterns that gave good discriminability. These analyses would further test

the validity of using race to determine the children’s dialect status and would allow us to

examine whether models developed on one subset of children could be used to classify

children in another subset. The dependent variable for all analyses was a percentage measure

(i.e., frequency of each pattern divided by the total number of complete and intelligible

utterances in each sample).

4 We also checked for, but did not find, dialect differences that were related to gender and SES Importantly, though, the limited

number of parent questionnaires that were returned, the restricted range of SES that was reported by those who returned the

questionnaires, and the unequal number of males and females in each group limited our ability to detect differences related to t\

hese

variables.

5 Initially, we completed the listener judgment tasks using three minutes of tape. When this was done, all three AA children were

identified as speakers of SAAE, but results were mixed for the one White child Exploration of listener judgment tasks as a way t\

o

classify a speaker’s dialect is on-going in our lab.

Oetting and McDonald Page 8

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Analyses of VarianceInitially, a three-way analysis of variance with dialect (SAAE vs. SWE) and child group

(SLI, 6N, 4N) as between-subjects factors and type of pattern (the 35 listed in Table 2) as a

within-subjects factor was run. All three main effects were significant. The main effect of

dialect reflected a higher percentage of coded patterns for the SAAE speakers (

M = 34.1%)

than for the SWE speakers (

M = 12.8%) [ F(l, 87) = 119.1, p < .001]. The main effect of

child group [

F(2, 87) = 5.7, p < .005] with subsequent Tukey post hoc tests showed a higher

percentage of coded patterns for the SLI group (

M = 27.6%) than either the 6N group ( M =

17.0%) or the 4N group (

M = 21.1%). Finally, the main effect of pattern type [ F(34, 2958) =

86.8,

p < .001] indicated that there was great variability in the rate at which the individual

patterns were produced. Across groups, the pattern with the highest percentage of use was

zero marking of

be (5.5%), and the pattern with the lowest percentage was I’ma (<.01%)

Both two-way interactions involving type of pattern also were significant: pattern type by

dialect [

F(34, 2958) = 30.6, p < .001] and pattern type by child group [ F(68, 2958) = 2.4, p

< .001]. These significant two-way interactions were further explored by running a 2

(dialect) × 3 (group) ANOVA on each nonmainstream pattern. Outcomes are reported in

Tables 4 and 5 as

F values, associated significance values, and eta squared values. Fourteen

of the patterns showed significant effects for dialect, with higher percentages of use

demonstrated by the SAAE children than by the SWE children (see Table 4). Note that these

patterns encompass verb agreement features with

be and don’t , zero-marking of many forms

(i.e., regular third-person present, irregular third-person present, copula and auxiliary

be

forms, regular past tense, plurals, possessives, of) and use of alternative or unique surface

expressions (i.e.,

be2 , had + past tense, multiple negation, use of a for an, and demonstrative

pronouns).

Six features showed significant group effects (see Table 5). Post hoc Tukey tests showed

that none of these features distinguished children with SLI from the 4N controls. Four

patterns, including zero marking of

be forms, zero marking of irregular past, omission of

auxiliary

do, and noninversion of Wh- questions, differentiated the SLI group from the 6N

group. For each of these patterns, rates of occurrence by the children with SLI were greater

than those of their age-matched peers. A slightly different set of four patterns (auxiliary

do

omission, zero marking of irregular past, zero marking of present progressive, and

appositive use) showed developmental trends between the 4N and 6N control groups. For

the first three patterns, rates of occurrence in the 4N group were greater than those in the 6N

group; for appositives, rate of use was higher for the 6N group than for the 4N group.

It is interesting to note that there was very little overlap in the patterns that differentiated the

two dialects from each other and those that differentiated the three child groups. In fact, only

zero-marking of

be was significant for both the dialect and group variables. Because there

was not a significant interaction between dialect and group for zero-marking of

be, the

effects of dialect and group status appear to be additive.

It also is important to note that in exploring the above interactions between pattern and

dialect and between pattern and group we performed multiple ANOVAs. One must always

be careful when interpreting the results of multiple tests to avoid finding spurious effects

that are actually due to inflated alpha error. However, we believe the effects that were

significant are of potential interest to other researchers who may wish to further explore

group differences on these features. The traditional way of adjusting for possible error is to

adopt a more restrictive alpha level for reporting significance, but this method carries the

risk of erroneously restricting the possible list of differentiating linguistic structures. A

supplemental source of information is eta, squared, a measure of variance accounted for. As

can be seen in Table 4, even with high within-group variability, two variables each account

Oetting and McDonald Page 9

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript for more than 40% of the variance across dialects. Also, as reported in Table 4, an alpha set

at .01 would identify SV agreement with

be (which accounted for 7% of the variance), but it

would not have identified multiple negation even though eta squared is 6%. Eta squared

values are lower in Table 5 (4%–10%), but recall that the group effects are being examined

with the dialects collapsed, so within-group variability is extremely high.

Discriminant Analyses Discriminant analysis is another way of determining which patterns allow for classification

of the children by dialect and which patterns allow for classification by group. In addition, if

patterns are intercorrelated, discriminant analyses will pick the best predictor and add

additional predictors only if they improve classification accuracy. The following

discriminant analyses were performed to see what patterns can be used to discriminate the

children by dialect and what patterns can be used to discriminate the children by group. Both

analyses considered only the SLI and 6N groups, following Bedore and Leonard (1998);

Dunn, Flax, Sliwinski, and Aram (1996); Fletcher and Peters (1984); and Gavin, Klee, and

Membrino (1993). According to Plante and Vance (1994), the ability to correctly classify

80% or more of the cases is considered fair, and the ability to correctly classify 90% or more

of the cases is considered good.

For both the analysis of dialect and the analysis of group, we started with a discriminant

function that included all 35 nonmainstream patterns to ascertain maximum group

discriminability. Thereafter, we ran stepwise discriminant analyses to get a model with a

reduced number of patterns that still gave reasonable discrimination. This type of analysis

adds, and deletes if appropriate, one pattern at a time until additional patterns do not

significantly improve discriminant performance. Weights are assigned to each pattern such

that the ability to distinguish between the groups is maximized. The entry and deletion

criterion for the current analyses was set to .10.

In order to test the generalizability of the discriminant functions, reduced models were

formed using only a subset of the participants and then these models were applied to the

remaining children to check the classification accuracy of members not used to set the

weights. For example, for the analyses seeking to classify children as speakers of either

SAAE or SWE, a discriminant function was set on the children with SLI and then this model

was applied to those in the 6N group, as well as vice versa. Similarly, for the analyses

seeking to classify children as either SLI or 6N, a discriminant function was set using SAAE

speakers and then this model was applied to SWE speakers and vice versa. Good

classification of new members indicates that a model is generalizable; poor classification

indicates that a model designed for a particular subgroup may not apply to others.

Dialect As mentioned earlier, children were independently classified as speakers of SAAE or SWE

on the basis of race. Use of discriminant analysis allows us to further examine whether this

grouping variable led to a differentiation of speakers by dialect. Indeed, a discriminant

function analysis that included all 35 nonmainstream patterns classified 97% of the speakers

into the correct dialect category. This full model could be substantially reduced without

much sacrifice in accuracy; a stepwise discriminant analysis yielded four features that

correctly classified 94% of all speakers. Table 6(a) provides details of the classification

accuracy of the full and reduced models. The four patterns in the reduced model, in order of

entry, were zero-marking of regular third, zero-marking of

be, subject-verb agreement with

be, and zero-marking of irregular past. These four patterns accounted for 72% of the

variance between the dialects. All four occurred more frequently in the SAAE dialect than in

the SWE dialect.

Oetting and McDonald Page 10

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Next, stepwise discriminant analyses to distinguish the two dialects were run separately over

the SLI group and 6N controls. The reduced model discriminating dialects based solely on

the SLI group yielded four patterns: zero-marking of

be, zero-marking of regular third,

subject-verb agreement with

be, and zero-marking of irregular past. Note that these features

were the same as those derived when both the SLI and 6N groups were considered together.

These patterns accounted for 62% of the variance between dialects in the SLI group. The

reduced model based solely on children with SLI correctly classified 81% of the children by

dialect type; see Table 6(b). When tested on the age-matched controls, this model correctly

classified everyone (100%), showing the stability of the model. The poorer performance on

the SLI group as compared to the 6N group, despite the fact that the weights were set on the

SLI group, indicates that the data from the SLI group were somewhat noisier than those of

their age-matched controls.

The reduced model based on the 6N controls alone also yielded four patterns: zero-marking

of regular third, zero-marking of

be, subject-verb agreement with be, and over-regularization

(e.g.,

breaked for broke ). Note that the first three patterns were in common with the model

based on the SLI group alone. This model accounted for 88% of the variance between

dialects in the 6N group. All features were more prevalent in SAAE than in SWE speakers.

This model correctly classified 97% of the age-matched controls and 74% of the children

with SLI; see Table 6(c). This model showed poorer transfer to the children with SLI

because of the over-regularization pattern. Indeed, when this pattern was eliminated from

the discriminant function, and weights reset over the 6N group, all age-matched controls

were correctly classified, and classification of the children with SLI climbed to 90%. Thus,

when discriminant function weights are set over one diagnostic group and then used to

classify members from the other, there is good discrimination of SAAE and SWE speakers.

This result indicates that the same patterns that distinguished the two dialects from each

other for normally developing children also apply to children with SLI.

Groups Children were independently classified into the SLI or 6N groups on the basis of criteria set

out in the Method section. A discriminant function considering all 35 nonmainstream

patterns was able to successfully mirror this classification at a 90% accuracy rate. A

stepwise discriminant analysis reduced the 35 patterns down to 4, while accuracy remained

fair at 82%. The 4 features were zero-marking of irregular past, auxiliary

do omission,

noninversion of Wh- questions, and over-regularization. For all four patterns, rates of use

were higher for the SLI group than for the 6N group. This model accounted for 50% of the

variance between the groups. This model had more trouble classifying SLI children correctly

(sensitivity of 74%) than age-matched controls correctly (specificity of 90%). Details of the

model’s performance are given in Table 7(a).

Next we examined whether the patterns that distinguish SLI from 6N for speakers of SAAE

also distinguish these diagnostic groups for speakers of SWE and vice versa. Good

classification across dialects would show the stability of the discriminant function and

would indicate that SLI manifests itself similarly in the two dialects. Poor classification in

one dialect with weights set from the other would indicate that the surface patterns of SLI

vary as a function of dialect.

The model to distinguish normal children from affected children that was fit solely on

speakers of SAAE had three patterns: zero-marking of irregular past, non-inversion of Wh-

questions, and zero-marking of irregular third. This model accounted for 45% of the

variance between the SAAE groups. SAAE speakers who were SLI were more likely to

zero-mark irregular past and to produce noninverted Wh- questions than their age-matched

peers, but they were less likely to produce zero-marked irregular third-person present forms.

Oetting and McDonald Page 11

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Although this model had fair accuracy (82%) when applied to SAAE speakers, the accuracy

rate fell to 71% when it was applied to SWE speakers. An examination of the performance

in Table 7(b) shows that the SAAE model had poor sensitivity (47%) and fair specificity

(89%). That is, applying the SAAE model to SWE speakers resulted in misclassifying many

SWE children with SLI as having normal language.

The model to distinguish normal children from impaired children that was fit solely on the

SWE speakers had five patterns; zero-marking of irregular past, auxiliary

do omission, zero-

marking of irregular third, omission of infinitive

to, and subject-verb agreement with don’t .

This model accounted for 72% of the variance between the SWE groups. For all five

patterns, SWE speakers with SLI had higher percentages of occurrence than SWE age-

matched controls. Although this model had good accuracy (91%) when applied to SWE

speakers, the accuracy rate fell to 61% when applied to SAAE speakers. As shown in Table

7(c), the sensitivity of the model was not bad (88%), but the specificity was extremely poor

(25%). That is, the SWE model tended to incorrectly classify normally developing SAAE

speakers as language impaired.

Discussion The SAAE and SWE speakers differed in the rate at which certain nonmainstream patterns

were used. This was evidenced by an interaction between dialect and pattern in the ANOVA

outcomes. Patterns involving subject-verb agreement features, zero-marking of many forms,

and the use of alternative or unique expressions occurred at a greater rate in SAAE than in

SWE, The discriminant analyses showed that four features, all verb-based, were sufficient to

give good discrimination between the dialects. Discriminant functions formed over one

subset of children, such as those in either the SLI or 6N group, also succeeded in classifying

children in the other subset. This pattern of results indicates that differences across the two

dialects are relatively stable, regardless of child profile. For models of impairment, this

finding provides evidence that children with SLI are extremely good at learning the

distributional properties of the dialect to which they are exposed.

The three child groups were also distinguished by the rate at which certain nonmainstream

patterns were used. An exploration of the interaction between group and pattern showed that

diagnostic category (SLI vs. 6N) differed on four patterns, whereas developmental

differences (4N vs. 6N) were present on four slightly different patterns. Interestingly, there

was very little overlap between patterns that differentiated the groups and those that

differentiated the dialects. This is an important finding because it shows that the SLI group

effect was not a result of these children’s being heavier dialect users overall than the

controls. Instead, on only a few specific nonmainstream patterns did the children with SLI

produce rates of use that were greater than their age-matched peers.

Another important finding came from the stepwise discriminant analyses that were run to

distinguish children with SLI from those developing language normally. Recall that

classification accuracies were fair to good when the dialects were examined separately.

Transfer of the reduced models across dialects, however, was very poor. This finding

indicates that the surface characteristics of SLI are influenced by the type of dialect spoken.

Thus, it is imperative that models of SLI be tested not only with a wide range of languages,

but also within the context of dialect diversity.

Even though differences in the SLI profile were observed, it is interesting that some patterns

related to tense marking showed effects for SLI in both dialects. Across analyses, these

included zero

be, zero irregular past, and zero irregular third. At least two other dialect

studies have reported tense-related problems for children with SLI. For example, when

Oetting and McDonald Page 12

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript calculating use by dividing the number of marked forms by total number of obligatory

contexts, Seymour et al. (1998) report a 41% difference (normal = 91% vs. impaired = 50%)

in overt past-tense marking between the normal and affected AAE-speaking children they

studied. In addition, using the same index of use as Seymour et al., overt marking of regular

third-person present was identified as difficult for SWE-speaking children with SLI in our

earlier work (normal = 95% vs. impaired = 72%; Oetting et al., 1999). Tense-related

weaknesses in children with SLI are consistent with standard English studies of SLI as well

as those that have examined SLI in other languages (Bedore & Leonard, 1998; Leonard,

Miller, & Gerber, 1999; Rice, Noll, & Grimm, 1997; Rice, Wexler, & Hersberger, 1998).

Thus, one important avenue for future work seems to be in the area of tense marking. Given

the different tense-marking patterns that have surfaced across the studies just reviewed, it

seems important to explore the effects of different indices of use on results as well as to

consider the full range of tense-marking options that are available in a given dialect(s).

Difficulties with question formation among the children with SLI also cut across the two

dialects. Recall that group effects (SLI vs. 6N) were observed for Wh- noninversion and

omission of auxiliary

do; the former pattern surfaced in the SLI reduced discriminant

function involving SAAE, and the latter surfaced in the reduced SLI model involving SWE.

Interestingly, work by Rice, Wexler, and Cleave (1995) with standard English speakers

found use of

be and do forma in questions to differentiate children with SLI from same-age

peers who were developing language normally. Craig and Washington (2000) also report

that African American English-speaking children with SLI perform lower than their age-

matched peers on tasks involving question comprehension. Thus, future work may want to

include question formation as a variable within studies of SLI. Like tense marking, however,

it is critical that one consider the full range of options children have when questions are

posed.

Findings related to tense marking and question formation suggest that, in at least some

cases, the basic mechanism underlying SLI may be the same across these two dialects, even

though the surface manifestations of the impairment differ. Findings related to zero marking

of irregular third person also warrant additional comment, because with this pattern surface

similarities of the SLI profile across these two dialects seem to be masking important

underlying differences. Recall that this pattern was found to discriminate children with SLI

from controls in both dialects, but distributions of use differed as a function of dialect type.

For SWE, children with SLI produced higher rates of zero marking than the controls; for

SAAE, this pattern was reversed.

Interestingly, when the number of obligatory contexts for irregular third person marking is

controlled (i.e., use is divided by number of obligatory contexts rather than number of

utterances) and rates of zero-marking are recalculated, findings for the SWE speakers do not

change; those with SLI are still found to zero-mark these forms at a greater rate (54%) than

their peers (4%). Findings for the SAAE speakers change, however, with the direction of the

group difference reversing (SAAE SLI = 73% zero-marked vs. SAAE 6N = 69%;

p > .05).

Inspection of the data indicate that those in the 6N group produced twice as many irregular

third contexts per sample (average = 4) than those with SLI (average = 2). Thus, unlike the

SWE speakers, the most pronounced difference between these two groups of SAAE

speakers is not in their rates of zero marking but in the frequency at which obligatory

contexts for this structure are produced. We speculate that this finding is related to the

normally developing SAAE speakers’ superior ability to use narrative discourse genre and

their use of historical present tense within these narrative contexts. If this is the case, then

future studies of SLI that are conducted within the context of dialect diversity will need to

carefully consider the role discourse plays in children’s use of morphosyntax.

Oetting and McDonald Page 13

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript In closing, it is noteworthy that children with SLI were distinguishable from those

developing language normally even though the analyses were completed with the contrastive

patterns of the target dialects. Recall from the literature review that contrastive patterns (i.e.,

those that differ from standard English) are often viewed as problematic within assessment

because they may be mistaken for a language deficit rather than a language difference. The

current findings do not suggest that current practice is necessarily misguided. In fact, data

presented in Table 2 can be used to reinforce the claim that surface characteristics of

nonmainstream English dialects often overlap with those reported as standard English

characteristics of SLI. What the current findings do suggest is that this overlap is at the level

of individual utterances and individual patterns. When children’s use of a full range of

nonmainstream patterns are considered and pattern use is treated as a continuous rather than

categorical variable, it is possible to distinguish different dialects and different child profiles

from each other. What, this means is that future studies of SLI can, and should, include the

contrastive as well as noncontrastive features of dialects.

The findings also indicate that future research is needed to improve the rate at which we are

able to correctly classify children as either SLI or normal. This work seems particularly

important for children who speak SAAE. Recall that only an 82% accuracy rate was found

for discriminating SAAE speakers with SLI from those developing normally. Although this

level of discrimination is considered fair from a statistical standpoint, it is lower than the

91% rate observed for the SWE speakers. One approach to improving classification

accuracy is to examine the profiles of the 5 SAAE speakers who were misclassified. Four of

the 5 children came from the SLI group, and 1 came from the 6N group.

Unfortunately, inspection of the data suggests nothing unusual about these five cases; their

testing data and rates of nonmainstream pattern use were consistent with those of their same-

dialect peer group. When individual patterns were examined, zero-marked

be was the only

one in which these 5 children seemed, to differ from their peers. The 6N child produced this

pattern at a rate of 12%, and this rate was slightly higher than the average (7.8%) of her

respective 6N group; the 4 misclassified children with SLI produced this pattern at rates of

6%–8%, and these rates were slightly lower than the average (10%) obtained by the SAAE

speakers with SLI. Unfortunately, zero-marking of

be did not surface within the reduced

discriminant function. Nevertheless, perhaps additional analyses along the lines of Wyatt

(1991) that take into account the effects of the preceding and following linguistic context of

each

be form will be useful for improving the classification accuracy of these children and

others in the future.

Another way to improve classification accuracy may be to consider adding noncontrastive

patterns to the discriminant function. Adding other language measures, such as MLU or use

of complex syntax, also may improve classification rates. Recently, both of these indices

have been found to differentiate AAE-speaking children with language impairments from

controls (Craig & Washington, 2000). Finally, there is some evidence that nonword

repetition tasks may be useful for identifying a language impairment, regardless of whether

a child is from a majority or minority culture (Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998; Oetting, Lynch,

Habans, Eyles, & Hall, 1999).

Research on SLI in the context of different dialects is sorely needed. One hopes that the

current findings will motivate others to test the replication and generalization of these

findings in other linguistically diverse, English-speaking communities. Work needs to

include participants from rural and urban settings, and dialects from different regions of the

country need to be examined. It also is important to examine alternative ways of classifying

children as dialect speakers, especially in communities where the overlap between the

variables of race and dialect is unclear.

Oetting and McDonald Page 14

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Of course, it is important to reiterate the preliminary and exploratory nature of the current

analyses and to restate the inherent limitations of using an existent data set to study

nonmainstream pattern use. In particular, effects of the examiner characteristics on the

children’s use of dialect could not be rigorously examined here. Also, small and unequal

numbers of sample sizes made it difficult to detect possible differences in dialect use that

were related to the participants’ gender and/or the educational level of the participants’

mothers. In fact, the current data set says very little about the complex relationship that

exists between dialect use and multiple sociodemographic variables because a stratified

sampling method was not used to select participants and information about whether the

children were part of open or closed social networks was not collected.

Finally, the current findings do not speak to the underlying grammatical representation of

SAAE and SWE. Our focus on surface pattern use, although helpful for thinking about the

ways in which morphosyntactic manifestations of a linguistic impairment may differ from

patterns of normal language variation, falls extremely short of providing a comprehensive

description of the two dialects under study. Qualitative studies and other sociolinguistic

research paradigms are needed to fully explore the unique and shared characteristics of these

two English varieties.

Acknowledgments The project was made possible by a grant from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication

Disorders (R03 DCO3609) that was awarded to the first author and an Interdisciplinary Research Summer Stipend

from Louisiana State University that was awarded to both authors Appreciation is extended to the families and staff

of the Ascension Parish school system who made the research possible, Jeannie Breaux who served as our SAAE

consultant, Lisa Green who shared with us her expertise of nonmainstream dialects of Louisiana, and Karen Pollock

who provided valuable comments regarding the design of the study and dissemination of the findings. Thanks also

are extended to the many students who have helped with various aspects of data collection and coding. These

include, but are not limited to, Amy Brock, Julie Cantrell, Cathy Cavell, Amy Costanza, Jennifer Depew, Lesley

Ellison, Lesley Eyles, Lesli Habans, Anita Hall, Janice Horohov, Dia McGowen, Myra Redlich, and Christy Wynn.

References Arwood, EL. Apricot I Language Kit. Portland, OR: Apricot; 1985.

Bailey, G.; Bassett, M. Invariant BE in the Lower South. In: Montgomery, M.; Bailey, G., editors. Language variety in the South. Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press; 1986. p. 158-179.

Bailey G, Bernstein C. The idea of Black English. The SECOL Review. 1990; 14:1–24.

Bailey G, Maynor N. The present tense of be in Southern Black folk speech. American Speech. 1985; 60:195–213.

Bailey G, Maynor N. Decreolization? Language and Society. 1987; 16:449–473.

Battle D. Language learning and use by African American children. Topics in Language Disorders. 1996; 16(4):22–37.

Baugh J. A survey of Afro-American English. Annual Review in Anthropology. 1983; 12:335–354.

Bedore L, Leonard L. Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998; 41:1185–1192.

Bernstein, C.; Nunnally, T.; Sabino, R., editors. Language variety in the South revisited. Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press; 1997.

Burgmeister, B.; Blum, H.; Lorge, I. Columbia Mental Maturity Scale. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1972.

Butters, R. The death of Black English: Divergence and convergence in black and white vernaculars. New York: Peter Long; 1989.

Craig H, Washington J. An assessment battery for identifying language impairments in African American children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2000; 43:366–379.

Dollaghan C, Campbell T. Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998; 41:1136–1146. Oetting and McDonald Page 15

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Dunn, ML.; Dunn, ML. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Revised. Circle Pines, MN: AmericanGuidance Service; 1981.

Dunn M, Flax J, Sliwinski M, Aram D. The use of spontaneous language measures as criteria for identifying children with specific language impairment: An attempt to reconcile clinical and

research incongruence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1996; 39:643–654. [PubMed:

8783141]

Faegin, C. The African American contribution to Southern States English. In: Bernstein, C.; Nunnally, T.; Sabino, R., editors. Language variety in the South revisited. Tuscaloosa: University of

Alabama Press; 1997. p. 123-139.

Fletcher P, Peters J. Characterizing language impairment in children: An exploratory study. Language Testing. 1984; 1:33–49.

Gavin W, Klee T, Membrino I. Differentiating specific language impairment from normal language development using grammatical analysis. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. 1993; 7:191–206.

Goldman, R.; Fristoe, M. Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1986.

Green, L. Unpublished dissertation. Amherst: University of Massachusetts; 1993. Topics in African American English: The verb system analysis.

Hollingshead, A. Four-factor index of social position. Yale University; 1975. Unpublished manuscript

Labov, W. Co-existent systems in African-American vernacular English. In: Mufwene, S.; Rickford, J.; Bailey, G.; Baugh, J., editors. African-American English: Structure, history, and use. New

York: Routledge; 1998. p. 110-153.

Leonard, L. Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998.

Leonard L, Miller C, Gerber E. Grammatical morphology and the lexicon in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999; 42:678–689.

Leonard L, Weiss A. Application of non-standardized assessment procedures to diverse linguistic populations. Topics in Language Disorders. 1983; 3:35–45.

Lucas, C.; Borders, D. Language diversity and classroom discourse. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corp; 1994.

McGregor K, Williams D, Hearst S, Johnson A. The use of contrastive analysis in distinguishing difference from disorder: A tutorial. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 1997; 6(3):

45–56.

Miller, J.; Chapman, R. Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts. Madison, WI: Language Analysis Laboratory, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin; 1992.

Montgomery, M.; Bailey, G., editors. Language variety in the South. Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press; 1986.

Mufwene, S.; Rickford, J.; Bailey, G.; Baugh, J., editors. African-American English: Structure, history, and use. New York: Routledge; 1998.

Myhill J. Postvocalic /r/ as an index of integration into the BEV speech community. American Speech. 1988; 63(3):203–213.

Myhill, J.; Harris, N. The use of the verbal -s inflection in BEV. In: Sankoff, D., editor. Diversity and diachrony. Amsterdam: John Benjamins; 1986. p. 25-31.

Newcomer, P.; Hammill, D. Test of Language Development–Primary. Austin, TX: ProEd; 1988.

Oetting J. Children with SLI make use of argument structure cues to learn novel verbs. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999; 42:1261–1274.

Oetting J, Cantrell J, Horohov J. A study of specific language impairment (SLI) in the context of nonstandard dialect. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. 1999; 13:25–44.

Oetting J, Horohov J. Past tense marking by children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1997; 40:62–74.

Oetting, J.; Lynch, K.; Habans, L.; Eyles, L.; Hall, A. Examining alternative test protocols for children from minority backgrounds. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American Speech

Language Hearing Association; San Francisco, CA. 1999.

Plante E, Vance E. Selection of preschool language tests: A data-based approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 1994; 25:15–24. Oetting and McDonald Page 16

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Rice M, Noll K, Grimm H. An extended optional infinitive stage in German-speaking children withspecific language impairment. Language Acquisition. 1997; 6:225–295.

Rice M, Wexler K, Cleave P. Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1995; 38:850–864.

Rice M, Waxler K, Hershberger S. Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.

1998; 41:1412–1431.

Rickford J. Ethnicity as a sociolinguistic boundary. American Speech. 1985; 60(2):99–124.

Rickford J, Ball A, Blake R, Jackson R, Martin N. Rappin on the copula coffin: Theoretical and methodological issues in the analysis of copula variation in African-American Vernacular English.

Language Variation and Change. 1991; 3:103–132.

Seymour, H. Clinical intervention for language disorders among nonstandard speakers of English. In: Taylor, O., editor. Treatment of communication disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse

populations. San Diego: College Hill Press; 1986. p. 135-152.

Seymour H, Bland-Steward L, Green L. Difference versus deficit in child African American English. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 1998; 29:96–108.

Spears A. Reassessing the status of Black English. Language and Society. 1992; 21:675–682.

Stockman I. The promises and pitfalls of language sample analysis as an assessment tool for linguistic minority children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 1996; 27:355–366.

Tager-Flusberg H, Cooper J. Defining the phenotype of SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999; 42:1275–1278.

Washington J, Craig H. Socioeconomic status and gender influences on children’s dialectal variations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998; 41:618–626.

Wolfram W. Re-examining Vernacular Black English. Language. 1990; 66:121–133.

Wolfram, W.; Schilling-Estes, N. American English. Maiden, MA: Blackwell; 1998.

Wyatt, T. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Massachusetts Amherst; 1991. Linguistic constraints on copula production in Black English child speech.

Appendix The criteria used to code the 35 patterns are listed below, examples are taken from the

normally developing children in this study Descriptions of 13 other nonmainstream patterns

that were considered but not included are available from the authors. Unless noted, all

patterns are described in the literature as possible in both dialects; however, most of the data

on these forms come from studies of AAE varieties.

Zero be: Zero-marking of copula and auxiliary structures regardless of contractibility,

persor, or number was counted (e. g., Oscar in the can). Although zero-marking of

be is rare

or infrequent in some contexts (e.g., with first-person pronouns, in finite contexts, clause

find positions, and in contexts with emphatic stress) and there is thought to be differences in

SAAE and SWE regarding the effect of these contexts on

be marking, all contexts were

coded here to examine the effects of the independent variables of interest.

Be 2: Instances where

be was produced to signify an event or activity distributed

intermittently over time or space, including auxiliary and copula contexts that refer to

durative or habitual meaning (e.g., It be on the outside). Utterances with omitted

will and

other standard English uses (e.g., I’m going to be a dalmation) were not included.

I'ma : Instances where

I’ma was produced instead of the standard English I’m going to (e.g.,

I’ma go peek and see if my class gone out that way). This pattern is mentioned in

discussions of reduced

gonna forms and is thought to occur in AAE varieties.

Oetting and McDonald Page 17

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Subject-verb agreement with be forms : Instances where the person and number of the be

form differed from its subject (e.g., When we was about to go to church).

Omission of auxiliary do: Instances where auxiliary

do was not produced, but in standard

English its presence is obligatory. Many of these instances involved question inversion (e.g.,

How you get up here? and What you did?). Questions with an omitted

do in the initial

position of the utterance (e.g., You know what? and You got a baby?) were not counted. See

discussion below about the coding of noninverted indirect requests/questions.

Omission of auxiliary have: Instances where auxiliary

have, has , and had was not

produced, but in standard English its presence is obilgatory (e.g., I only been there a few

times). As demonstrated by the example, many of these utterances involved the verb

been .

Zero regular third present : Instances where regular third-person marking on the verb was

zero-marked (e.g, But when she poo on herself I don’t change her). Decisions as to whether

present or past tense was implied by the child were based on context.

Zero irregular third present : Instances where the subject of the verbs

say, have , and do

required says, has , and does in standard English but the child produced the unmarked form

(e.g., She just do it herself). Utterances involving

don’t were not included because they were

counted elsewhere. For the verb say, all zero-marked forms were coded as third present

irregular. For some of these utterances, the child’s meaning may have been past rather than

present. The decision to include all of the say examples as present was based on the

children’s frequent use of historical present with the verb say (e.g, So she says stop it!).

Within the sociolinguistic literature, a distinction between regular versus irregular verb

forms is not always made, although some (like Myhill & Harris, 1986) exclude the verb say

in analyses because it is irregular and typically zero-marked.

Subject-verb agreement with don't: Instances where the subject of the verb required

doesn’t in standard English, but the child produced don’t (e.g., And he don’t go to school).

Zero regular past : Instances where unmarked verbs were produced and in standard English

simple past marking is obligatory (e.g., I dress them before). Adjectival readings also were

included because they are included in sociolinguistic research (e.g., It’s finish).

Zero irregular past : Instances where an irregular verb was zero-marked for past tense (e.g.,

fall for fell ) or a different past-tense form was used instead of a standard English form (e.g.,

Course I brung him up real fast). In some cases, the different verb form was the participle

(e.g., I seen it).

Had + past : Instances where

had + a past-tense verb was produced and the standard English

gloss would be the simple past or the past participle (e.g., One day I had went on the back of

the levee to the beach). This pattern has been reported for SAAE.

Over-regularization : Instances where regular past-tense marking was used with an

irregular verb form (e.g., She drinked it all). This pattern is thought to occur infrequently in

both dialects.

Past as participle : Instances where the simple past-tense form was produced and in

standard English a participle form is required (e.g., But her whole head got broke).

BIN and been: Stressed

BIN and unstressed been contexts were included. BIN contexts

were those where the event was thought to be on-going or the completive activity is in the

remote past (e.g., Because I

BIN having them for a bunch of times. And I BIN had shots).

Oetting and McDonald Page 18

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Seven of the utterances reflect clear examples of BIN as confirmed by Green (personal

communication). The other 8 are less clear, two may reflect

BIN , at least four can be glossed

with was, one may be a past-tense form of

be2 , and two may reflect omission of have . Been

uses involving clear cases of zero-marked have were not included in this category but were

included as instances of zero

have (see above). BIN is thought to be an AAE feature.

Ain't : Instances where

ain’t was used and in standard English negative forms involving be,

do

, or have are obligatory (e.g., We ain’t got none).

Multiple negation : Instances where negation was marked more than once in the utterance

(e.g., Cause she don’t want no people on the rocks). This pattern often occurs with

don’t and

ain’t .

Indefinite article : Instances where indefinite article

a was used and the following context

involves a vowel (e g., It’s a animal story). This pattern is thought to occur in SAAE.

Zero present progressive : Instances where present progressive inflection was zero-marked

and in standard English overt marking is obligatory (e.g., Yep I’m build one of those).

Zero plural : Instances where the regular plural inflection was zero-marked and in standard

English overt marking is obligatory (e.g., Six dollar and fifty-five). This pattern is thought to

occur most frequently with nouns of weights and measures or with nouns preceded by

quantification.

Zero possessive : Instances where the possessive inflection was zero-marked and in standard

English overt marking is obligatory (e.g, We’ll probably need everybody plates).

Omission of infinitive to: Instances where infinitive

to was omitted. Omission of to as a

preposition was not included (e.g., “My sister asked me if I wanted her bake some cookies

with the sugar”).

For to/to : Instances where

for to was produced and in standard English infinitive to is

produced. Only two instances of this pattern were found in the data, and both may be

considered questionable (e.g., I mean for to take a walk. For to go to store and pay).

Zero of : Instances where the preposition of was omitted (e.g., I can’t tell too much the story

yet).

What for that or zero that : Instances where the relative pronoun what was produced (e.g.,

Anything what my momma brings) or the relative pronoun was omitted (e.g., And they had

that thing you gotta shift your money in). Relative pronouns in the subject and object

position were included even though absence of that occurs in some standard English object

clauses.

Done + verb : Instances where

done + verb indicated a completive action or event (e.g. He’s

looking for his cat but it done went down the garbage can).

Fixing + verb : Instances where

fixing and fitna were used as a main verb and followed by

an infinitive (e.g., He was fixing to go off of the roof like that). One instance of

might gotta

(e.g., I might gotta take you somewhere) also was included in this category.

Undifferentiated pronoun : Instances where the unmarked pronoun form was used instead

of standard English nominative (e.g., Me and him do it sometimes), use of nominative

Oetting and McDonald Page 19

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript marking instead of genative (e.g., they cat), and use of masculine forms for feminine (e.g.,

he do it).

Reflexive : Instances where a different reflexive pronoun form was produced instead of a

standard English form (e.g., My daddy once went by hisself because he didn’t want to be

worried about us).

Demonstrative : Instances where the objective pronoun form was produced instead of the

demonstrative (e.g., He wrecked them back tires).

Dative : Instances where a personal dative was produced (e.g., I take me a shot).

Y'all varieties : Instances where a variant of a second-person plural form was produced

instead of a standard English pronoun (e.g., Y’all take turns).

Appositive : Instances where both a pronoun and noun were used to refer to the same

person(s) or object (s) (e.g., But my friend, he have a gate). This pattern occurs in standard

English but is thought to be more frequent in SAAE and SWE varieties.

Existential it and they: Instances where

it or they was used instead of there (e.g., My dad

grabs it with a paddle whenever it’s only men).

Wh- noninversion : Instances where a Wh- question form began the utterance or clause, but

the auxiliary was not inverted (e.g., Why this one won’t sit).

Oetting and McDonald Page 20

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 21 Table 1

Participant characteristics. SLI6N4N SAAESWESAAESWESAAESWEN 161512191219 Males 912513512 Age in months77.1 (6)76.3 (8)74.5 (4)76.1 (6)56.83 (3)48.3 (5)Average level of education a 3.62 (1.40)4.67 (.50)5.00 (0.00)4.60 (.99)3.63 (.74)5.11 (.99) 89315817 CMMS b 95.8 (5)98.6 (6)101.3 (4)104.3 (9)102.4 (7)104.7 (10)PPVT c 71.4 (10)73.9 (10)102.2 (13)104.9 (12)97.8 (8)102.2 (7)TOLD III d 5.0 (2)6.3 (2)8.8 (3)8.7 (2)10.0 (2)9.0 (2)TOLD IV6.1 (1)5.7 (2)12.1 (2)11.4 (3)9.4 (2)9.6 (1)TOLD V6.1 (1)6.7 (2)9.9 (3)12.2 (3)9.3 (2)9.7 (1)GFTA e 73.8 (21)66.6 (29)95.6 (6)92.8 (13)80.1 (15)88.8 (8)MLU-w f 4.43 (.9)4.41 (.7)5.49 (1.5)5.27 (.8)4.65 (.5)4.41 (.48)MLU-m g 4.75 (.9)4.83 (.7)5.90 (1.6)5.80 (.8)4.98 (.6)4.85 (.6)Mean C&I utterances h 188 (52)248 (87)221 (79)242 (47)192 (63)204 (38)Total C&I utterances i 300337252652461323023876

aScores based on Hollingshead (1975), numbers below standard deviations reflect the number of children who returned a questionn\

aire.

bStandard scores from the Columbia Mental Maturity Scale (Burgmeister, Blum, & Lorge, 1972): M = 100, SD = 15.

cStandard scores from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Revised (Dunn & Dunn, 1981): M = 100, SD = 15.

dStandard subtest scores from the Test of Language Development–Primary (Newcomer & Hammill, 1988), Subtest III = Grammatical Co\

mprehension, IV = sentence Repetition, V = Sentence Completion:M = 10, SD = 3.

ePercentiles from the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation (Goldman & Fristoe, 1986),

fMean length of utterance calculated by words.

gMean length of utterances calculated by morphemes.

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25. NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 22 hAverage number of complete and intelligible utterances per sample.

iTotal number of complete and intelligible utterances per group. J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25. NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 23 Table 2

Frequency of nonmainstream dialect patterns. SLI6N4N SAAESWESAAESWESAAESWEzero be3091612116324689be2513373341 I’ma for I’m going to 002000 SV agreement with be 392935252325omission of auxiliary do 525614224260omission of auxiliary have 2941214 zero regular third13542112258519zero irregular third 2927327268 SV agreement with don’t 382523233633zero regular past 46312853310zero irregular past 40401563134had + past22053080 over-regularization 12201171134participle as past 1113243 ain’t 271315231015multiple negation 422425302935indefinite article 6662152 zero present progressive 118471116zero plural 39171515215 zero possessive 239279146 zero infinitive to 14181271213for to/to 000110 zero of 231112121212what/that or zero that 445255 been and BIN 213225 done + verb000101 fixing + verb204211

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25. NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 24 SLI6N4N SAAESWESAAESWESAAESWEundifferentiated pronoun 614116141925reflexive 612253 demonstrative 1000230 dative 2642113 y’all varieties624227 appositive 252937491316existential it and they 010200 Wh- noninversion 262673712

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25. NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 25 Table 3

Rate of nonmainstream pattern use.

a

SAAESWE SLI6N4NSLI6N4NM .37.31.34.18.09.13SD .13.09.13.08.04.06General range b .24–.60.19–.47.22–.67.06–.22.03–.18.05–.25Outliers c male .17nonefemale .11male .42nonenonefemale .17

aSum of nonmainstream pattern use divided by total number of complete and intelligible utterances.

bThe general ranges are based on 89 of the 93 children. Excluded are the 4 children who are presented as outliers.

cFour children are presented as outliers because their rates of nonmainstream dialect fell outside those of their same-dialect ch\

ild group; the gender of each outlier is provided for descriptive purposes.

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25. NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 26 Table 4

Patterns that differ by dialect. PatternSignificanceEta SquaredSAAE MSWE Mzero be F(1, 87) = 74.6, p < .001 .429.42.5 be2F(1, 87) = 21.6, p < .001 .191.6.1 SV agreement with be F(1, 87) = 7.1, p < .01 .071.3.6 zero regular thirdF(1, 87) = 69.0, p < .001 .434.6.7 zero irregular thirdF(1, 87) = 19.3, p < .001 .171.1.3 SV agreement with don't F(1, 87) = 8.4, p < .005 .081.3.7 zero regular pastF(1, 87) = 23.0, p < .001 .191.5.4 had + past F(1, 87) = 8.7, p < .005 .091.20.0 multiple negationF(1, 87) = 5.6, p < .05 .061.2.7 indefinite articleF(1, 87) = 13.3, p < .001 .12.4.2 zero pluralF(1, 87) = 9.1, p < .005 .091.0.3 zero possessiveF(1, 87) = 17.5, p < .001 .16.8.2 zero of F(1, 87) = 11.0, p < .005 .11.6.3 demonstrativeF(1, 87) = 61, p < .05 .06.2.01

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25. NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 27 Table 5

Patterns that differ by group. Mean PatternSignificanceEta SquaredSLI6N4Nomission of auxiliary do F(2, 87) = 4.8, p < .05 .10 1.4a.4 b1.6a zero irregular pastF(2, 87) = 7.1, p < .005 .14 1.4a.3b1.1a zero be F(2, 87) = 3.7, p < .05 .04 7.2a3.9 b5.4ab Wh- noninversionF(2, 87) = 4.3, p < .05 .09 .7a.1b.3ab zero present progressiveF(2, 87) = 3.5, p < .05 .07 .3ab.2a.4bappositiveF(2, 87) = 3.3, p < .05 .07 .8ab1.2a.4b

aMeans with different superscripts are significantly different by Tukey post hoc tests.

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25. NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 28 Table 6

Discriminant analysis models distinguishing between dialects. Overall

accuracy in

classificationAccurate

classification of SAAEAccurate

classification SWE (a) Models with weight set over SLI and 6N groups combined Full model 60/6228/28 32/34 Reduced model a 58/6227/28 31/34 (b) Reduced model set on SLI only b Applied to SLI 25/3113/16 12/15 Applied to 6N 31/3112/12 19/19 (c) Reduced model set on 6N only c Applied to 6N 30/3111/12 19/19 Applied to SLI 23/3114/16 9/15

aReduced model included zero regular third, zero be, subject-verb agreement with be, zero irregular past (72% of variance explained).

bReduced model included zero be, zero regular third, subject-verb agreement with be, zero irregular past (62% of variance explained).

cReduced model included zero regular third, zero be, subject-verb agreement with be, overregularization (88% of variance explained).

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25. NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Oetting and McDonaldPage 29 Table 7

Discriminant analysis models distinguishing between SLI and 6N groups. Overall

accuracy in

classificationAccurate

classification

of SLI groupAccurate

classification of 6N group (a) Models with weights set over SAAE and SWE dialects combined Full model 56/62 27/3129/31 Reduced model a 51/62 23/3128/31 (b) Reduced model set on SAAE only b Applied to SAAE 23/28 12/1611/12 Applied to SWE 24/34 7/1517/19 (c) Reduced model set on SWE only c Applied to SWE 31/34 13/1518/19 Applied to SAAE 17/28 14/163/12

aReduced model included zero irregular past, auxiliary do omission, Wh- noninversion, overregularization (50% of variance explained).

bReduced model included zero irregular past, Wh- noninversion, zero irregular third (45% of variance explained).

cReduced model included zero irregular past, auxiliary do omission, zero irregular third, omission of infinitive to, subject-verb agreement with don’t (72% of variance explained).

J Speech Lang Hear Res . Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 June 25.