Read McFarland, Brown, and Webb (2013) downloadarticle before completing this discussion board. According to the authors, the key to decreasing prejudice and intergroup conflict is identification wit

http://cdp.sagepub.com/ Science Current Directions in Psychological http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/22/3/194 The online version of this article can be found at:   DOI: 10.1177/0963721412471346 2013 22: 194 Current Directions in Psychological Science Sam McFarland, Derek Brown and Matthew Webb Identification With All Humanity as a Moral Concept and Psychological Co\ nstruct     Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of:     Association for Psychological Science can be found at:

Current Directions in Psychological Science Additional services and information for         http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts:   http://cdp.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions:   http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints:   http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions:   What is This?  - Jun 4, 2013 Version of Record >> by guest on March 4, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from by guest on March 4, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from Current Directions in Psychological Science 22(3) 194 –198 © The Author(s) 2013 Reprints and permissions:

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721412471346 cdps.sagepub.com In July 1940, after the Nazis had conquered one half of Poland and the Soviets the other half, masses of refugee Jews crowded the gates of foreign embassies and consul- ates in Lithuania, pleading for protection. Although most diplomatic outposts, including the American embassy, turned them away, those who came to the one-man Japanese consulate in Kaunas were fortunate. Chiune Sugihara, the Japanese diplomat there, saw many “beg- ging with tears in their eyes . . . They were so desperate that they went so far as to kiss my shoes” (quoted in Levine, 1996, p. 259). Throughout August, and contrary to his government’s orders, Sugihara wrote thousands of visas for Jews to travel to Japan, often working 18 hours a day. He knew that doing so could cost him his career, which it did. When the Soviets conquered Lithuania and closed all foreign consulates, Sugihara and his family had to leave on September 5th on a train bound for Berlin.

However, he continued to hand out visas until the train departed. His wife later estimated that Sugihara had saved 6,000 Jews (Sugihara, 1990). Levine (1996) esti- mated 10,000, as many of the visas Sugihara wrote were for entire families. When he later described his motives, Sugihara wrote, “I acted according to my sense of justice, out of love for mankind” (quoted in Levine, 1996, p. 253). We began with Sugihara’s story because he exempli- fied identification with all humanity, a deep caring for all human beings regardless of their race, religion, or nationality. Sugihara saw only suffering people who needed his help; he did not distinguish these desperate Jews from anyone else. Later interview studies with those who rescued Jews during the Holocaust reported that the most decisive quality distinguishing them from nonrescu- ers was that they possessed a sense “of belonging to one human family” (Monroe, 1996, p. 205)—a concern for others that extended across all boundaries of race and religion (Oliner & Oliner, 1988). That identification with all humanity is the topic of the research reported here. The Historical Development of Identification With All Humanity Historically, the concept of one human family has devel- oped slowly since the late 15th century. The great geo- graphic discoveries of America, Africa, and Asia unleashed the brutalization and enslavement of their inhabitants but also created “the incipient notion of the human race as a single collectivity” (Headley, 2008, p. 27). That notion 471346 CDP XX X 10.1177/0963721412471346McFarland et al.Identification With Humanity research-article 2013 Corresponding Author:

Sam McFarland, Department of Psychology, 1906 College Heights Blvd., Western Kentucky University, Bowling Green, KY 42101-1030 E-mail: [email protected] Identification With All Humanity as a Moral Concept and Psychological Construct Sam McFarland 1, Derek Brown 2, and Matthew Webb 1 1Department of Psychology, Western Kentucky University, and 2comScore, Inc., Reston, Virginia Abstract Studies of those who rescued Jews during the Holocaust suggest that thei\ r most shared quality was a sense “of belonging to one human family” (Monroe, 1996, p. 205), caring deepl\ y about human beings without regard for their race, religion, or other distinction. In this article, we first note the\ development of the concept of “one humanity” since the late 15th century, and then we summarize recent work with a new meas\ ure of that caring—the Identification With All Humanity Scale (IWAH). Research with the IWAH establishes that ide\ ntification with all humanity is more than (a) an absence of prejudice and its sources and (b) the sum of posit\ ive qualities, such as dispositional empathy and principled moral reasoning. Many people appear to intuit that a mature m\ oral person would identify with all humanity, even when they do not do so themselves. Finally, a brief discussion is offered of how identification with all humanity may develop or could be taught.

Keywords identification with humanity, ethnocentrism, human rights, authoritarian\ ism, social dominance, empathy by guest on March 4, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from Identification With Humanity 195 e volved slowly. It was expressed, for example, in the 18th and 19th centuries in the struggles to end slavery. Only in the 20th century, however, do we find a surge of self- conscious expressions of all humanity as a single group, such as the saying widely attributed to Gandhi, “All humanity is one undivided and indivisible family” or Steichen’s (1955) The Family of Man. With this emerging moral sense of a common human- ity, humanity became a focus of international law in the 20th century. The concept of “crimes against humanity,” coined early in the century, was used first as a legal con- cept in the Nürnberg trials after World War II (Clapham, 2007). When the United Nations General Assembly (1948) declared in the Universal Declaration of Human Rights that human rights belong to all human beings, “without distinction of any kind, such as race, color, sex, language, religion, political or other opinion, national or social ori- gin, property, birth or other status” (Article 2), the com- mon humanity of all human beings was created as a moral mandate as never before. The concept of crimes against humanity was slowly incorporated into interna- tional law in the latter half of the 20th and early 21st centuries (McFarland, 2011).

Identification With All Humanity as an Individual Difference This historical development notwithstanding, a sense of identification with all humanity is today an individual dif- ference worthy of study. It is more than an absence of prejudice, as one could be free of all prejudices but still care little for humanity. To Alfred Adler and Abraham Maslow, identification with all humanity was an expression of human maturity.

To Adler, gemeinschaftsgefuhl (social interest) was a gen- uine caring for others that, in its most mature form, leads to activities that are useful in their “helpfulness to all mankind, present and future” (Adler, 1927/1954, p. 78). To Maslow, individuals who have attained “self-actualization” have a deep “feeling of identification with mankind,” think of themselves as “members at large of the human species,” and have “a genuine desire to help the human race” (Maslow, 1954, p. 138). Adler did not create a measure of mature social interest, and Maslow did not create one of self-actualization. Several later efforts were made to measure these constructs (e.g., Crandall, 1980; Shostrom, 1964), but these measures assess other facets of the constructs and not identification with all humanity (McFarland, Webb, & Brown, 2012). Because identification with all humanity is an impor - tant moral concept, and no measure of it existed, we developed the Identification With All Humanity Scale (IWAH; McFarland et al., 2012). The measure consists of nine three-response items in the following form: 1. Ho w much do you identify with (that is, feel a part of, feel love toward, have concern for) each of the following?

a. P eople in my community b. Americans c. All humans e verywhere 2. When the y are in need, how much do you want to help:

a. P eople in my community b. Americans c. P eople all over the world Responses are on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). The sum of the “c” items consti- tutes the IWAH. The full measure and further information on the scale’s development are available elsewhere (see McFarland et al., 2012, and http://edtech.wku.edu/ ~smcfar/documents/iwah.pdf ). By presenting the three identifications together, a comparison is implied but not requested. When used in other countries, that country’s name is used instead of “Americans” (e.g., Hamer & Gutowski, 2009). For most participants, identification with all humanity is not high. Participants in our many adult and student samples average almost exactly 3 (somewhat). About 10% average 2 (just a little) or less; fewer than 10% aver - age 4 (quite a bit) or higher. The mean for each IWAH item is usually about half a point lower than the mean of the other two identifications, and fewer than 15% iden- tify as much with all humanity as with the other two groups. Because the three identifications correlate positively— those who say that they care about “all humans every- where” are also likely to say that they care about their closer groups—and the concern is to understand the unique associations with caring about “all humans every- where,” the other identifications are used as statistical controls. For an explanation of this statistical control, see McFarland et al. (2012). McFarland et al. (2012) reported a series of 10 studies testing the validity of the IWAH. Identification with all humanity was consistently negatively related as expected to generalized prejudice (the tendency to have many prejudices, often called ethnocentrism), as measured by Altemeyer’s (1996) Manitoba Ethnocentrism Scale, and to its two main roots (McFarland, 2010), right-wing authori- tarianism (Altemeyer, 1996) and the social dominance orientation (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). The IWAH was pos- itively related to dispositional empathy (Davis, 1983) and principled moral reasoning (Rest, Narvaez, Thoma, & Bebeau, 1999), and it was weakly related to self-rated political liberalism. However, statistically, all of these together explained just a portion of the IWAH. From the six factors of the HEXACO model (Ashton & Lee, 2009), by guest on March 4, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from 196 McFarland et al.

c onsisting of the well-known Big Five personality traits plus an added morality factor, identification with all humanity was positively related to Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism, but these explained only a small portion of the IWAH. Across sev- eral samples, the IWAH was unrelated to self-rated reli- giousness or religious conservatism. The IWAH was found to be generally stable in individuals across 10 weeks.

Others who know an individual well tend to score them similarly on the IWAH to how they score themselves. The IWAH consistently predicted concern about global issues (e.g., global warming), concern about humanitar - ian needs, and support for universal human rights on measures developed by McFarland and Mathews (2005), including a willingness to invest national resources and to send troops to defend people around the world in situ- ations such as genocide or ethnic cleansing. Individuals high on the IWAH were found to have greater knowledge of global humanitarian issues, and they were more likely to choose to read about them over other matters (McFarland et al., 2012). They were more prone to value the lives of outgroup members (Afghanis in this study) equally with the lives of ingroup members (Americans) on a measure of the ethnocentric valuation of human life (Pratto & Glasford, 2008). Individuals high on the IWAH consistently pledged larger contributions for international humanitarian relief. The IWAH predicted these findings even when the related constructs cited in the previous paragraph (generalized prejudice, empathy, etc.) were statistically controlled. Members of a major humanitarian charity and of a major human rights organization scored substantially higher on the IWAH than a general adult sample. The IWAH was also found (a) to be distinct from measures of the importance of one’s morality (Aquino & Reed, 2002) and Schwartz’s (1992) value of universalism and (b) to predict concern for universal human rights beyond these measures (McFarland et al., 2012). It appears that many individuals can intuit identifica- tion with all humanity as a moral ideal. McFarland and Brown (2008) asked a large sample of students, after they had completed the measure, to complete it a second time as “the most mature and most moral person you could imagine anyone being would answer” (McFarland & Brown, 2008). The IWAH scores for 86% of the partici- pants were higher for this most mature and moral person than they had reported for themselves. Whereas the indi- viduals’ own mean on the IWAH items was 3.03 (some- what), their mean for the most mature and moral person was 4.03 (quite a bit). Just 7% of this sample averaged 4.0 or higher in their personal IWAH responses, but 45% thought that a fully mature and moral individual would do so ranging from 4 (quite a bit) to 5 (very much).

Those high in right-wing authoritarianism and the social dominance orientation were somewhat less likely than others to envision identification with all humanity as a moral ideal.

These results suggest a kind of moral intuition, that many persons who have never thought about identifying with all humanity can nonetheless intuit doing so as an important moral ideal. This moral intuition appears to differ from the moral intuitionism popularized by Haidt (2001). Haidt emphasized primitive emotions that drive moral judgments (e.g., the deep feeling that it is wrong to eat one’s dead pet), even when one cannot offer a logical reason for the judgment. In contrast, the belief that one should identify with all humanity appears logically com- pelling rather than emotionally driven. Second, Haidt’s moral intuitionism emphasizes moral feelings that per - sons possess and claim, whereas McFarland and Brown’s (2008) participants intuited that a fully mature and moral person would possess a morality that they, themselves, did not (McFarland & Brown, 2008). This issue merits further study. General Discussion Research with the IWAH confirms that identification with all humanity is more than (a) the absence of gen- eralized prejudice and its roots and (b) the positive qualities of dispositional empathy, principled moral rea- soning, and other related constructs. Although these qualities and identification with all humanity are consis- tently related, identification with all humanity predicts concern for human suffering and human rights— valuing the lives of ingroup and outgroup members more equally, knowledge of humanitarian concerns, and giving to international charity beyond these related constructs. For us, the critical issue is to learn how identification with all humanity develops. Its roots in heredity, child - rearing, and later life experiences are not known. In an unpublished study, an effort was made to identify memo- ries of childrearing that correlate with identification with all humanity. A large adult sample completed 54 items with the stem, “When I was a child, my parents, on the whole . . .” These items measured seven dimensions, including affection and support (“were very affection- ate”), moral and caring (“had concern for suffering peo- ple”), intellectual and global (“encouraged me to think about global issues”), punitive (“used physical punish- ment quite a bit”), religious (“wanted me to be devoutly religious”), patriotic (“were very patriotic”), and spoiling (“were very lenient”). Alas, none of the seven dimensions related to the participants’ identification with all human- ity; nothing of their parents’ childrearing that we mea- sured was found to contribute to it. We offer further by guest on March 4, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from Identification With Humanity 197 speculations on the sources of identification with all humanity (McFarland et al., 2012).

Can identification with all humanity be taught? There appear to be few public efforts to do so. In the 1970s, a children’s television series, Big Blue Marble, named for a photo of the Earth surrounded by black space, tried to teach it. Its theme song contained the refrain, Folks are folks and kids are kids, we share a common name, We speak a different way but work and play the same. (Redwine & Paris, 1973) We know of no similar effort today, as patriotism and ingroup loyalty appear to dominate the public values now taught to children. Perhaps, in addition to children’s TV shows, the story of Chiune Sugihara and stories of others like his can inspire this identification. Perhaps the moral intuition found by McFarland and Brown (2008) offers a possibil- ity: Merely presenting the question of how a moral and mature person would think regarding all humanity might inspire many to realize that identifying with all humanity is a moral value they should adopt as their own. The issue of how identification with all humanity develops or could be taught merits serious study. If it can be, our research suggests that we might witness a greater con- cern for many of humanity’s problems—from genocide to world hunger to global warming. Readers will undoubtedly find limitations in this research and potential new directions for studying iden- tification with all humanity. We sincerely hope that others will address these with their own studies.

Recommend Reading Headley, J. M. (2008). (See References). Headley traces the evolution of “the idea of humanity as a single moral col- lectivity” (p. 63) from the early Renaissance (c. 1500) to the creation of the Universal Declaration of Human Rights in 1948.

McFarland, S., Webb, M., & Brown, D. (2012). (See References). This article provides more detail on the development and validity of the Identification With All Humanity Scale.

Monroe, K. (1996). (See References). This study of rescuers of Jews during the Holocaust inspired our studies of identifi- cation with all humanity.

Oliner, S., & Oliner, P. (1988). (See References). This study of rescuers of Jews during the Holocaust inspired our studies of identification with all humanity.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. References Adler, A. (1954). Problems of neurosis. New York, NY: Harper Torchbooks. (Original work published 1927) Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Aquino, K. F., & Reed, A., II. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1423–1440. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1423 Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO-60: A short measure of the major dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 340–345. doi:10.1080/ 00223890902935878 Clapham, A. (2007). Human rights: A very short introduction.

New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Crandall, J. E. (1980). Adler’s concept of social interest: Theory, measurement, and implications for adjustment.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 481–495.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.481 Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113 Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment.

Psychological Review, 108, 814–834. doi:10.1037/0033- 295X.108.4.814 Hamer, K., & Gutowski, J. (2009). Social identifications and pro- social activity in Poland. In S. Scuzzarello, C. Kinnvall, & K. R. Monroe (Eds.), On behalf of others: The psychology of care in a global world (pp. 163–183). New York, NY:

Oxford University Press.

Headley, J. M. (2008). The Europeanization of the world: On the origins of human rights and democracy. Princeton, NJ:

Princeton University Press.

Levine, H. (1996). In search of Sugihara: The elusive Japanese diplomat who risked his life to rescue 10,000 Jews from the Holocaust. New York, NY: Free Press.

Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

McFarland, S. (2010). Authoritarianism, social dominance, and other roots of generalized prejudice. Political Psychology, 31, 425–449. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00765.x McFarland, S. (2011). Presidential address: The slow creation of humanity. Political Psychology, 32, 1–20. doi:10.1111/ j.1467-9221.2010.00801.x McFarland, S., & Brown, D. (2008). Who believes that identifi- cation with all humanity is ethical? Psicologia Politica, 36, 37–49.

McFarland, S., & Mathews, M. (2005). Who cares about human rights? Political Psychology, 26, 365–386. doi:10.1111/ j.1467-9221.2005.00422.x McFarland, S., Webb, M., & Brown, D. (2012). All humanity is my ingroup: A measure and studies of identification with all humanity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 830–853.

Monroe, K. (1996). The heart of altruism: Perception of a common humanity. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

by guest on March 4, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from 198 McFarland et al. Oliner, S., & Oliner, P. (1988). The altruistic personality: Rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe. New York, NY: Free Press.

Pratto, F., & Glasford, D. E. (2008). Ethnocentrism and the value of a human life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1411–1428. doi:10.1037/a0012636 Redwine, S., & Paris, N. (1973). Big blue marble original theme song. Santa Monica, CA: A & M Records.

Rest, J., Narvaez, D., Thoma, S. J., & Bebeau, M. J. (1999). DIT2: Devising and testing a new instrument of moral judgment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 644–659.

doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.644 Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content of structure and values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). doi:10.1016/S0065- 2601(08)60281-6 Shostrom, E. L. (1964). An inventory for the measurement of self- actualization. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 24, 207–218. doi:10.1177/001316446402400203 Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Steichen, E. (1955). The family of man. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

Sugihara, Y. (1990). Rokusen Nin No Inochi No Viza [Visas for life for 6,000 people]. Orien, Japan: Asahi.

United Nations General Assembly. (1948). The Universal Declaration of Human Rights. Retrieved from United Nations Web site: http://www.un.org/en/documents/udhr/ by guest on March 4, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from