can someone write 2 summaries for the 2 articles attached? the summaries need to be no more than 250 words. summaries should include the background, hypotheses, studies, results, and a brief discussi

Addressing Neuroticism in Psychological Treatment Shannon Sauer-Zavala, Julianne G. Wilner, and David H. Barlow Boston University Neuroticism has long been associated with psychopathology and there is increasing evidence that this trait represents a shared vulnerability responsible for the development and maintenance of a range of common mental disorders. Given that neuroticism may be more malleable than previously thought, targeting this trait in treatment, rather than its specific manifestations (e.g., anxiety, mood, and person- ality disorders), may represent a more efficient and cost-effective approach to psychological treatment.

The goals of the current manuscript are to (a) review the role of neuroticism in the development of common mental disorders, (b) describe the evidence of its malleability, and (c) review interventions that have been explicitly developed to target this trait in treatment. Implications for shifting the focus of psychological treatment to underlying vulnerabilities, such as neuroticism, rather than on the manifest symptoms of mental health conditions, are also discussed.

Keywords:neuroticism, temperament, treatment Neuroticism is typically defined as the tendency to experience frequent and intense negative emotions in response to various sources of stress. While the emotions considered within the pur- view of this trait include the range of negative affect (e.g., fear, irritability, anger, and sadness), the greatest focus has been on the experience of anxious and depressive mood. The perception that the world is a dangerous and threatening place also accompanies this exaggerated negative emotionality, along with beliefs about one’s lack of agency to handle challenging events. Manifestations of this trait may include heightened focus on criticism, either self-generated or from others, as confirming a general sense of inadequacy and perceptions of lack of control over salient events (Barlow, 2002;Barlow, Sauer-Zavala, Carl, Bullis, & Ellard, 2014;Clark & Watson, 2008;Eysenck, 1947;Goldberg, 1993).

Barlow and colleagues (Barlow et al., 2014) have forwarded a model to account for the development of neuroticism informed by an interaction of biological and psychological vulnerabilities. To summarize, the general biological risk is evidenced by high heri- tability estimates (40 to 60%) for this trait (e.g.,Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001;Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994;Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003), with genetically mediated neuroticism linked to the neurobiological tendency for heightened reactivity in emotion-generating structures, including amygdala hyperexcitabil- ity, and reduced inhibitory control by prefrontal systems (Keight- ley et al., 2003;Stein, Simmons, Feinstein, & Paulus, 2007; Westlye, Bjørnebekk, Grydeland, Fjell, & Walhovd, 2011). These neural circuits are also influenced by a general psychological vulnerability that includes stressful, traumatic, or related develop- mental experiences during childhood, cultivating the perceptionthat life events are unpredictable and uncontrollable (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007;Lanius, Frewen, Vermetten, & Yehuda, 2010; Rosen & Schulkin, 1998). Taken together, these general biological and psychological vulnerabilities build upon each other to produce the neurotic phenotype. More important, these interacting vulner- abilities were originally described as part of an etiological model for trait anxiety and emotional disorders more generally (Barlow, 2000,2002), suggesting that a neurotic temperament may be a necessary component for the development of a range of psycho- logical conditions. In fact, all the temporal covariance among the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders(DSM; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) constructs of depression and several anxiety disorders can be accounted for by neuroticism (Brown, 2007).

Neuroticism has been associated with a wide range of public health problems (see:Lahey, 2009). For example, this trait strongly predicts a variety of mental disorders as well as comor- bidity among them (Clark et al., 1994;Khan, Jacobson, Gardner, Prescott, & Kendler, 2005;Krueger & Markon, 2006;Weinstock & Whisman, 2006), including personality disorders (Henriques- Calado, Duarte-Silva, Junqueira, Sacoto, & Keong, 2014;Sauer- Zavala & Barlow, 2014;Widiger, Verheul, & van den Brink, 1999). Neuroticism has also been associated with a range of physical problems including cardiovascular disease, eczema, asthma, and irritable bowel syndrome (Brickman, Yount, Blaney, Rothberg, & De-Nour, 1996;Smith & MacKenzie, 2006;Suls & Bunde, 2005). Additionally, this trait predicts treatment seeking and response to treatment for both mental disorders and general health concerns (Shipley, Weiss, Der, Taylor, & Deary, 2007).

Considering these public health consequences, it is not surprising that the economic cost of this trait exceeds the cost of common mental disorders (Cuijpers et al., 2010).

A discussion of the public health costs associated with neurot- icism prompts consideration of how this trait may be addressed. As noted above, the transactional relationship between genetic and environmental inputs for neuroticism suggests that the inherited contributions of this trait may serve as a predisposition, but not a Shannon Sauer-Zavala, Julianne G. Wilner, and David H. Barlow, De- partment of Psychological and Brain Science, Boston University.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shannon Sauer-Zavala, Center for Anxiety and Related Disorder, Department of Psychological and Brain Science, Boston University, 648 Beacon Street, Boston, MA 02215. E-mail:[email protected] This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment© 2017 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 8, No. 3, 191–1981949-2715/17/$12.00http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000224 191 mandate. This opens the question of whether neuroticism can be treated directly, rather than separately targeting each manifestation of this trait in the form of separate mental health and physical disorders. This approach is consistent with National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative that challenges researchers to look beyond diagnoses to identify core processes implicated in the development and maintenance of symptoms across a range of disorders (Insel et al., 2010). Targeting neuroticism itself may represent a more efficient and cost-effective means of addressing the wide swath of public health problems associated with it. Malleability of Neuroticism Although personality traits have long been considered stable and inflexible across time (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), there is increasing evidence that neuroticism may be more mal- leable than originally believed. For example, in a review of the literature regarding the stability of personality disorders,Clark (2009)notes that the traits associated with these diagnoses do change slowly over time, with the greatest shifts occurring in the behavioral manifestations of these traits. Further, longitudinal studies of the general population also show gradual age-related decreases in neuroticism and related constructs (i.e., internalizing) that continue into old age (Eaton, Krueger, & Oltmanns, 2011; Roberts & Mroczek, 2008;Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006). Overall, this pattern of results suggests that, on average, neuroticism decreases across time; however, when change in neu- roticism has been examined at the individual level using growth modeling (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003), there appears to be great variability in the extent of change, with some people maintaining a stable level of this trait and others changing considerably (Hel- son, Jones, & Kwan, 2002;Small, Hertzog, Hultsch, & Dixon, 2003). Indeed, there is evidence to suggest that individuals with higher initial levels of neuroticism tend to show less change in this dimension over time, and conversely, individuals with lower initial levels of neuroticism tend to evidence greater change (Brown, 2007).

Change in neuroticism has also been explored in the context of individuals seeking treatment forDSMdisorders. For example, in a study that tracked 41 individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) across 8 months (most of whom received some kind of treatment during this time), neuroticism remained stable, despite changes in clinical state (Eaton et al., 2011). Specifically, neurot- icism displayed the same high level of temporal stability in indi- viduals who no longer met criteria for MDD as it did in the subgroup of participants who were depressed at both assessment points. In contrast, others have indeed found changes in neuroti- cism as a function of time and treatment. For example,Brown and colleagues (2007;Brown & Barlow, 2009) examined the temporal course of neuroticism and its relationship to theDSM–IVdisorder constructs of depression, social anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder in a large sample (N 606) of outpatients with these disorders. Participants were assessed at intake and then reassessed at 1-year and 2-year follow-ups with the majority (76%) of patients receiving some kind of treatment (of varying quality and duration) during the follow-up period. As expected,DSM–IV disorder constructs improved significantly over time and other temperamental variables (e.g., extraversion) remained stable; neu-roticism, however, evidenced the greatest amount of temporal change and was the dimension associated with the largest treat- ment effect.

In addition to studies exploring the malleability of neuroticism in treatment-seeking samples where the treatment received was unspecified, changes in neuroticism have also been examined in the context of specific interventions. First,Kring, Persons, and Thomas (2007)assessed the degree to which neuroticism (de- scribed as negative affectivity in this study) and symptoms of depression and anxiety changed across a course of cognitive– behavioral therapy. Results suggest that neuroticism, as well as symptoms of depression and anxiety, decreased following the 12-week course of treatment; unfortunately, it is difficult to deter- mine whether change in temperament is driving change in symp- toms or vice versa as the study did not include multiple repeated measures of these variables. Additionally, in a large randomized- controlled trial of cognitive therapy (CT) compared with placebo for adults with major depressive disorder (Tang et al., 2009), CT produced greater changes in both neuroticism and extraversion than placebo. However, contrary toBrown’s (2007)results, after controlling for changes in depressive symptoms, the effect of CT on these temperamental variables remained significant only for extraversion. Finally, levels of neuroticism did not appear to change significantly after a course of treatment with dialectical behavior therapy for individuals with borderline personality dis- order (Davenport, Bore, & Campbell, 2010).

The inconsistent results described above may be because of the fact that the measurement of neuroticism consists of some combi- nation of stable temperamental variance and variability attributable to generalized distress that is apt to covary with temporal fluctu- ations in the severity of disorders. While this measurement issue, known as mood state distortion, is one possible explanation of the diverse findings with regard to temperamental malleability, there is also a considerable body of compelling research to suggest that these fluctuations represent actual changes in the level of neurot- icism secondary to situational stressors. Given that neuroticism is chiefly defined as the tendency to experience negative emotions, it is reasonable to consider mood state fluctuations that affect self- reported levels of neuroticism as direct expressions of this trait (Costa, Bagby, Herbst, & McCrae, 2005;Widiger & Smith, 2008).

Indeed, results from a recent study indicate that measures of neuroticism primarily capture true temperamental variance even in individuals with emotional disorders (Naragon-Gainey, Gallagher, & Brown, 2013).

Similarly, to the extent that high neuroticism is necessary for the development of a range of common mental disorders, one would expect that this trait would vary in accordance with the onset or remission of symptoms. In fact, a number of longitudinal studies have controlled for the periodic occurrence of anxious or depres- sive symptoms and still found neuroticism to act independently in predicting anxiety and mood (Lahey, 2009;Spijker, de Graaf, Oldehinkel, Nolen, & Ormel, 2007). Further, while neuroticism predicts the courseDSMdisorders, with higher levels of this trait reflecting less change in symptoms across time, the converse does not appear to occur; that is, initial levels ofDSMdisorders do not predict changes in temperament over time (Brown & Rosellini, 2011;Gershuny & Sher, 1998;Kasch, Rottenberg, Arnow, & Gotlib, 2002; Meyer, Johnson, & Winters, 2001). This pattern of This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 192 SAUER-ZAVALA, WILNER, AND BARLOW results, personality traits predicting symptoms but not vice versa, has also been found for personality disorders (Warner et al., 2004).

In summary, there appears to be evidence that neuroticism is subject to change slowly over across the course of the life span (e.g.,Clark, 2009;Roberts & Mroczek, 2008); however, the degree to which this and other temperamental variables are malleable in response to treatment remains unclear. The studies described above examined changes in neuroticism in the context of natural- istic treatments or treatments targeting disorder-specific symptoms and have yielded mixed results. Indeed, the research reviewed raises questions about the mechanisms through which neuroticism changes and whether directly targeting this trait in treatment would lead to more definitive results. Treatment of Neuroticism Most studies that have examined changes in temperament in response to psychological treatment, including the studies de- scribed in the previous section, utilize interventions that were not specifically designed to target neuroticism itself, but rather to addressDSMdisorder symptoms. It is possible that the lack of a priori specification regarding how and why these treatments im- pact temperament may have led to the mixed findings on the responsiveness of neuroticism to treatment; in other words, some interventions may be more suited to address neuroticism than others. Emerging research, however, suggests that when neuroti- cism is addressed directly with interventions explicitly designed to target it, change on this construct is more robust. This literature, including specific interventions for neuroticism and the putative pathways through which they target it, are reviewed below.

Most of the studies that examine interventions specifically de- signed to target temperament have come from the psychopharma- cology literature (for review, see:Ilieva, 2015;Soskin, Carl, Alp- ert, & Fava, 2012). Serotonergic drug agents (i.e., selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors) appear to produce dampening effects on neuroticism (Fu et al., 2004;Harmer, Mackay, Reid, Cowen, & Goodwin, 2006;Harmer et al., 2009;Murphy, Yiend, Lester, Cowen, & Harmer, 2009;Quilty, Meusel, & Bagby, 2008), while catecholaminergic (i.e., noradrenergic/dopaminergic) drugs en- hance extraversion (McCabe, Mishor, Cowen, & Harmer, 2010; Tomarken, Dichter, Freid, Addington, & Shelton, 2004). Clear hypotheses relating these agents to neurobiological changes have been forwarded that underscore their effect on temperament. For example, serotonergic agents likely produce dampening of neurot- icism because of their ability to decrease hyperreactivity of the amygdala in response to fear-inducing stimuli and to inhibit do- paminergic neurotransmission in areas of the prefrontal cortex. In contrast, catecholaminergic agents may be associated with in- creased extraversion because of their ability upregulate noradren- ergic and dopaminergic neurotransmission, particularly within the mesolimbic reward circuitry (Soskin et al., 2012). The fact that specific pharmacological agents display preferential effects on dimensions of temperament suggests that there may be utility in designing behavioral treatments to selectively address tempera- mental variables.

Currently, there are few behavioral interventions that are explic- itly designed to target temperament. One of the first examples of a treatment developed to address temperamental vulnerabilities, rather than psychological symptoms, isRapee, Kennedy, Ingram,Edwards, and Sweeney (2005)intervention for behavioral- inhibited children; the goal of addressing behavioral inhibition was to prevent the onset of future anxiety disorders. The program focuses on parenting training to minimize augmentation of the child’s biological nature through environmental interactions and includes psychoeducation about the nature of anxiety, traditional cognitive– behavioral strategies (i.e., exposure and cognitive re- structuring) directed toward personal concerns, and training in behavior management techniques that prevent an overprotective parenting style. Results from randomized controlled trials (Rapee et al., 2005;Rapee, Kennedy, Ingram, Edwards, & Sweeney, 2010) suggest that this program is indeed successful at preventing anx- iety disorders. Although a brief version of this program did not appear to produce significant changes in behavioral inhibition measured via parent report or laboratory observation (Rapee et al., 2010), a more intensive version with higher risk children indeed led to significantly greater reductions in this trait compared with those who did not receive the treatment (Kennedy, Rapee, & Edwards, 2009). Differences among groups increased over time, leadingRapee et al. (2010)to speculate that more intensive inter- ventions directed at temperament might produce an increasing trajectory of change in temperament compared with addressing more surface-level disorder symptoms.

The Unified Protocol for Transdiagnostic Treatment of Emo- tional Disorders (UP;Barlow et al., 2011) represents another example of an intervention explicitly developed to address tem- peramental vulnerabilities, in this case neuroticism, associated with common mental health conditions. As noted above, neuroti- cism is implicated in the development of the range of anxiety, depressive, and related disorders (e.g., somatic symptom disorders, trauma-related disorders), also known as “emotional disorders” (Barlow, 2002) given the role of strong emotions in their devel- opment. In addition to frequently occurring negative emotions, emotional disorders are also characterized by aversive reactions to emotional experiences, leading to problematic efforts to escape or avoid them (see:Barlow et al., 2014;Sauer-Zavala & Barlow, 2014). To address neuroticism, the UP consists of six core treat- ment modules aimed at extinguishing distress in response to the experience of strong emotions. The rationale for this aim is that with fewer aversive reactions to negative emotions when they occur, reliance on avoidant emotion regulation strategies that ex- acerbate symptoms is reduced, which in turn leads to less frequent and intense negative emotions over time. This approach has shown efficacy for the range of anxiety and unipolar depressive disorders (Barlow et al., 2016;Boswell, Anderson, & Barlow, 2014;Ellard, Fairholme, Boisseau, Farchione, & Barlow, 2010;Farchione et al., 2012) and there is preliminary support for the use of the UP with bipolar disorder (Ellard, Deckersbach, Sylvia, Nierenberg, & Bar- low, 2012) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD;Gallagher, 2015).

In addition, the emotional disorders framework has been ex- tended BPD and the related phenomena of suicidal and nonsuicidal self-injurious behavior (Bentley, Nock, & Barlow, 2014;Bentley et al., 2016;Sauer-Zavala & Barlow, 2014). The presentation of BPD is largely accounted for by neuroticism, with 4 out of 7 traits (emotional lability, anxiousness, separation insecurity, and depres- sivity) included in the inDSM–5’s alternative model of BPD stemming from the parent factor of neuroticism from the Five Factor Model (FFM) of personality (American Psychiatric Asso- This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 193 TREATMENT OF NEUROTICISM ciation, 2013). Additionally, the impulsive behaviors that charac- terize BPD, including suicidal and nonsuicidal self-injury, are often conceptualized as maladaptive attempts to reduce or escape from intense negative affect (Chapman, Gratz, & Brown, 2006).

Although potentially more life-threatening, these BPD-related be- haviors serve a similar function (relief from negative emotions) as leaving a social situation might for an anxiety patient or taking a nap for a depressed patient. Consequently, skills from the UP targeting the aversive, avoidant reactions to emotions that maintain both neuroticism and symptoms of emotional disorders are also relevant for BPD. In fact, the UP or its skill modules have been used to successfully treat BPD (Sauer-Zavala, Bentley, & Wilner, 2016), nonsuicidal self-injury (Bentley, Nock, Sauer-Zavala, Gor- man, & Barlow, 2016), and suicidal thoughts and behaviors (Bent- ley et al., 2016).

The modules of the UP have been described in detail elsewhere (e.g.,Payne, Ellard, Farchione, Fairholme, & Barlow, 2014); how- ever, a summary of how the six core modules cultivate a more accepting stance toward emotional experiences is described here.

First, by providing information about the adaptive, functional nature of emotions in the psychoeducation module (core module 1), patients begin to cultivate the stance that emotions provide useful information and should not be avoided. Next, patients receive instruction onhowto engage willingly, versus with avoid- ance, with their emotional experiences via mindfulness training (core module 2); specifically, patients are taught the benefits of a present-focused, nonjudgmental attitude toward their emotions through three complimentary experiential exercises.

Following the presentation of the UP orientation (e.g., cultivat- ing a more accepting stance toward emotions), patients are taught specific skills that map onto three interacting components of an emotional experience (i.e., thoughts, behaviors, and physical sen- sations). First, patients are encouraged to be more flexible in the way they appraise emotion-eliciting situations (core module 3); patients are instructed to question the automaticity of interpreta- tions, rather than to change maladaptive cognitions—in line with the UPs emphasis on acceptance. The following module, counter- ing emotional behaviors (core module 4), involves the identifica- tion of patient-specific avoidance behaviors that hinder full expo- sure to strong emotions. Patients are then instructed to act counter to their emotion-driven behavioral urges by engaging in activities that may put them in contact with strong emotions in the short- term. Next, patients cultivate a greater tolerance of physical sen- sations through interoceptive exercises (e.g., hyperventilation, breathing through a thin straw) that deliberately provoke the phys- iological feelings associated with strong emotions (core module 5).

Finally, treatment culminates with emotion exposures (core mod- ule 6) in which patients engage in a series a series of activities that elicit strong or uncomfortable emotions. Through this practice, patients’ aversive reactions to emotions are gradually extinguished via new learning that emotions are temporary and can be tolerated.

There is evidence to suggest that the approach to targeting temper- ament utilized in the UP indeed leads decreases in aversive reactions to emotions that in turn leads to changes in negative affectivity (Sauer-Zavala et al., 2012). Additionally, in the context of a small randomized-controlled trial (i.e.,Farchione et al., 2012), the UP produced small to moderate effects on measures of neuroticism from pre- to posttreatment compared with a waitlist group, and these changes were maintained at the 6-month follow-up assessment point(Carl, Gallagher, Sauer-Zavala, Bentley, & Barlow, 2014). In addi- tion, these changes in neuroticism predicted decreased anxiety and depressive symptoms and reductions in functional impairment. Over- all, the results of this investigation suggest in a preliminary manner that neuroticism can be successfully targeted in treatment and that reductions in this temperamental vulnerability also affect treatment outcomes.

More recently,Armstrong and Rimes (2016)conducted a pilot study examining the effect of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT;Segal, Teasdale, & Williams, 2002) that had been explicitly modified to directly target levels of neuroticism. The version of MBCT used in this study included references to neuroticism-related constructs, rather than depression-related themes. For example, Ses- sion 1 covered stress-reactivity by introducing the fight-or-flight re- sponse, the role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and un- helpful ways of responding to stress (i.e., overthinking and avoiding).

Session 2 involved discussion about the relationship between thoughts and feelings as well as common interpretation biases, and in Session 3, genetic and environmental contributions that make an individual susceptible to stress are discussed. In Session 4 patients learn about the long- and short-term consequences of avoiding stress-eliciting situations and Session 5 extends this discussion by presenting the notion that avoiding such situations leads to increased suffering.

Sessions 6 and 7 cover additional maladaptive, neurotic responses to stress including overthinking and self-criticism. Finally, Session 8 ties together the skills for “stress management” that had been learned previously in the treatment. After 8-weeks of treatment, participants in the MBCT condition demonstrated significantly greater reductions in neuroticism than participants in the Internet-based self-help control condition. Conclusions Contrary to theoretical conceptions of personality, research sug- gests that neuroticism may be malleable over time or in response to treatment; however, such findings have been mixed, indicating that additional work is necessary to identify the specific conditions that impact temperament. Interventions that were explicitly developed to target neuroticism appear to have the most consistent effects on this trait. Rapee and colleagues’ (Rapee et al., 2005,2010) work with behaviorally inhibited children addresses the neuroticism by targeting the environmental factors (e.g., parenting styles) that contribute to the general psychological vulnerability (e.g., the belief that the world is a dangerous, uncontrollable place;Barlow et al., 2014) necessary for this trait’s development. In adults, both the UP and neuroticism- focused MCBT address neuroticism by targeting the aversive, avoidant reactions to emotions that, paradoxically, increase the fre- quency and intensity of future negative emotions (Armstrong & Rimes, 2016;Carl et al., 2014). In sum, these efforts provide prelim- inary support for the notion that psychological interventions can indeed address temperamental vulnerabilities, and that such improve- ments are associated with a range of beneficial treatment outcomes (Carl et al., 2014;Farchione et al., 2012;Kennedy et al., 2009).

There are several advantages to shifting the focus of treatment to core temperamental vulnerabilities, rather than focusing disorder- specific symptoms. First, the rates of comorbidity among common mental disorders, including emotional disorders and personality disorders, are quite high (Grant et al., 2008;Zanarini et al., 1998).

Rather than prioritizing treatment of one condition over another, an This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 194 SAUER-ZAVALA, WILNER, AND BARLOW intervention focused on a shared vulnerability (e.g., neuroticism) could lead to simultaneous improvement across symptoms of all co-occurring disorders. Additionally, separate treatments for each Diagnostic and Statistical Manual diagnosis places a burden on practitioners to receive training to competently provide these in- terventions; targeting common temperamental mechanisms may reduce the number of treatments, increasingly the likelihood that empirically supported treatments will be more widely dissemi- nated.

Although there are there are theoretical advantages to address- ing neuroticism directly in treatment, there are practical implica- tions associated with moving away from the field’s disorder- specific focus. For example, it is customary, and often necessary, to provide aDSMdiagnosis (rather than levels of neuroticism) for psychotherapy to be reimbursed by insurance companies. This issue is addressable as targeting disorder symptoms and the core temperamental factors that maintain them are not mutually exclu- sive; the goal of addressing neuroticism in treatmentisremission ofDSMdisorders. There may be benefit, however, for insurance companies to shift focus from presence or absence of a particular disorder to levels dimensional variables, like personality traits, as the latter may be more informative with regard to an individual’s functioning (Widiger & Trull, 2007). Additional research is nec- essary to establish clinically significant thresholds for neuroticism that would point to whether treatment should be applied or if meaningful enough gains have been to warrant ending treatment.

Additionally, it is important to note that all three behavioral treatments developed to target neuroticism consist of multiple components. Given that they have each been tested only in their entirety, it is unclear whether each component contributes equally to the observed reductions in neuroticism. Future research should explore the unique contributions of each skill included in these treatment packages to identify the components that lead to the most robust and efficient changes in temperament. In addition, future research must also explore the possibility of intervention before a neurotic temperament fully develops. In fact, there is evidence to suggest that behavioral inhibition can be detected as early as 4 months old from crying and salivary cortisol in response to novel stimuli (Moehler et al., 2008). The animal literature provides insight into strategies that may be useful in preventing neuroticism; for example, exposing newborn rats to novel envi- ronments for 3 min a day resulted in less behavioral inhibition (defined as time spent in exploration after weaning) compared with newborn rats that stayed in their home cage (Tang, Reeb- Sutherland, Romeo, & McEwen, 2012). As such, it is important to identify early biological and behavioral markers that may indicate a vulnerability for the development of a neurotic temperament and that could serve as triggers for intervention.

In summary, treatments developed to directly target neuroticism indeed appear to impact this trait. However, to our knowledge, only three behavioral trials have been published with the aim of impacting temperamental factors, despite the advantages conferred by targeting shared vulnerabilities instead of disorder symptoms. Future research should not only continue to develop treatments for temperament but also explore which components included in these treatments for neuroticism are the most successful at reducing this trait, as well as how early interventions can occur. References American Psychiatric Association. (2013).Diagnostic and statistical man- ual of mental disorders(5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Armstrong, L., & Rimes, K. A. (2016). Mindfulness-based cognitive ther- apy for neuroticism (stress vulnerability): A pilot randomized study.

Behavior Therapy, 47,287–298.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2015 .12.005 Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory.American Psychologist, 55, 1247–1263.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1247 Barlow, D. H. (2002).Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Barlow, D. H., Ellard, K. K., Fairholme, C., Farchione, T. J., Boisseau, C., Allen, L., & Ehrenreich-May, J. (2011).Unified protocol for the trans- diagnostic treatment of emotional disorders. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Barlow, D. H., Farchione, T. J., Bullis, J. R., Gallagher, M. W., Latin, H., Sauer-Zavala, S.,...Cassiello-Robbins, C. (2016).Equivalence eval- uation of the Unified Protocol for Transdiagnostic Treatment of Emo- tional Disorders compared to diagnosis-specific CBT for anxiety disor- ders. Manuscript in preparation.

Barlow, D. H., Sauer-Zavala, S., Carl, J. R., Bullis, J. R., & Ellard, K. K.

(2014). The nature, assessment, and treatment of neuroticism: Back to the Future?Clinical Psychological Science, 2,344 –365.http://dx.doi .org/10.1177/2167702613505532 Bentley, K. H., Nock, M. K., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). The four-function model of non-suicidal self-injury: Key directions for future research.

Clinical Psychological Science, 2,638 – 656.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 2167702613514563 Bentley, K. H., Nock, M. K., Sauer-Zavala, S., & Gorman, B. S. (2016).

Examining the effects of two transdiagnostic, emotion-focused interven- tions on non-suicidal self-injury using single-case experimental design.

Manuscript submitted for publication.

Bentley, K. H., Sauer-Zavala, S., Cassiello-Robbins, C., Conklin, L. R., Vento, S. & Homer, D. (2016).Treating suicidal thoughts and behaviors within an emotional disorders framework: Acceptability and feasibility of the unified protocol in an inpatient setting. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Boswell, J. F., Anderson, L. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). An idiographic analysis of change processes in the unified transdiagnostic treatment of depression.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82,1060 – 1071.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037403 Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and personality.Behavior Genetics, 31,243–273.http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/ A:1012294324713 Brickman, A. L., Yount, S. E., Blaney, N. T., Rothberg, S. T., & De-Nour, A. K. (1996). Personality traits and long-term health status. The influ- ence of neuroticism and conscientiousness on renal deterioration in type-1 diabetes.Psychosomatics, 37,459 – 468.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1016/S0033-3182(96)71534-7 Brown, T. A. (2007). Temporal course and structural relationships among dimensions of temperament andDSM–IVanxiety and mood disorder constructs.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116,313–328.http://dx .doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.313 Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (2009). A proposal for a dimensional classification system based on the shared features of theDSM–IVanx- iety and mood disorders: Implications for assessment and treatment.

Psychological Assessment, 21,256 –271.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ a0016608 Brown, T. A., & Rosellini, A. J. (2011). The direct and interactive effects of neuroticism and life stress on the severity and longitudinal course of depressive symptoms.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120,844 – 856.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023035 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 195 TREATMENT OF NEUROTICISM Carl, J. R., Gallagher, M. W., Sauer-Zavala, S. E., Bentley, K. H., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). A preliminary investigation of the effects of the unified protocol on temperament.Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55,1426 – 1434.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.04.015 Chapman, A. L., Gratz, K. L., & Brown, M. Z. (2006). Solving the puzzle of deliberate self-harm: The experiential avoidance model.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44,371–394.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat .2005.03.005 Clark, L. (2009). Stability and change in personality disorder.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18,27–31.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01600.x Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (2008). Temperament: An organizing paradigm for trait psychology. In J. P. Oliver, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.) Handbook of personality: Theory and research(3rd ed., pp. 265–286).

New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,103–116.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.103.1.103 Costa, P. T. Jr., Bagby, R. M., Herbst, J. H., & McCrae, R. R. (2005).

Personality self-reports are concurrently reliable and valid during acute depressive episodes.Journal of Affective Disorders, 89,45–55.http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.06.010 Cuijpers, P., Smit, F., Penninx, B. W. J. H., de Graaf, R., ten Have, M., & Beekman, A. T. F. (2010). Economic costs of neuroticism: A population- based study.Archives of General Psychiatry, 67,1086 –1093.http://dx .doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.130 Davenport, J., Bore, M., & Campbell, J. (2010). Changes in personality in pre- and post-dialectical behaviour therapy borderline personality disor- der groups: A question of self-control.Australian Psychologist, 45, 59 – 66.http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00050060903280512 Eaton, N. R., Krueger, R. F., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2011). Aging and the structure and long-term stability of the internalizing spectrum of per- sonality and psychopathology.Psychology and Aging, 26,987–993.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024406 Ellard, K. K., Deckersbach, T., Sylvia, L. G., Nierenberg, A. A., & Barlow, D. H. (2012). Transdiagnostic treatment of bipolar disorder and comor- bid anxiety with the unified protocol: A clinical replication series.

Behavior Modification, 36,482–508.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0145445512451272 Ellard, K. K., Fairholme, C. P., Boisseau, C. L., Farchione, T. J., & Barlow, D. H. (2010). Unified protocol for the transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders: Protocol development and initial outcome data.

Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 17,88 –101.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1016/j.cbpra.2009.06.002 Eysenck, J. J. (1947).Dimensions of personality. Oxford, England: Kegan Paul.

Farchione, T. J., Fairholme, C. P., Ellard, K. K., Boisseau, C. L., Thompson-Hollands, J., Carl, J. R.,...Barlow, D. H. (2012). Unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders: A ran- domized controlled trial.Behavior Therapy, 43,666 – 678.http://dx.doi .org/10.1016/j.beth.2012.01.001 Fu, C. H., Williams, S. C., Cleare, A. J., Brammer, M. J., Walsh, N. D., Kim, J.,...Bullmore, E. T. (2004). Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: A prospec- tive, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Ar- chives of General Psychiatry, 61,877– 889.http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archpsyc.61.9.877 Gallagher, M. W. (2015).Unified protocol for posttraumatic stress disor- der. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Gershuny, B. S., & Sher, K. J. (1998). The relation between personality and anxiety: Findings from a 3-year prospective study.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107,252–262.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.107.2 .252Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits.

American Psychologist, 48,26 –34.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003- 066X.48.1.26 Grant, B. F., Chou, S. P., Goldstein, R. B., Huang, B., Stinson, F. S., Saha, T.D.,...Ruan, W. J. (2008). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity ofDSM–IVborderline personality disorder: Results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions.The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69,533–545.http://dx .doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v69n0404 Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development.Annual Review of Psychology, 58,145–173.http://dx.doi .org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605 Harmer, C. J., Mackay, C. E., Reid, C. B., Cowen, P. J., & Goodwin, G. M.

(2006). Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues.Biological Psychiatry, 59,816 – 820.http://dx .doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.015 Harmer, C. J., O’Sullivan, U., Favaron, E., Massey-Chase, R., Ayres, R., Reinecke, A.,...Cowen, P. J. (2009). Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166,1178 –1184.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020149 Helson, R., Jones, C., & Kwan, V. S. (2002). Personality change over 40 years of adulthood: Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of two longi- tudinal samples.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 752–766.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.752 Henriques-Calado, J., Duarte-Silva, M. E., Junqueira, D., Sacoto, C., & Keong, A. M. (2014). Five-factor model personality domains in the prediction of Axis II personality disorders: An exploratory study in late adulthood women non-clinical sample.Personality and Mental Health, 8,115–127.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1250 Ilieva, I. (2015). Enhancement of healthy personality through psychiatric medication: The influence of SSRIs on neuroticism and extraversion.

Neuroethics, 8,127–137.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12152-014-9226-z Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., . . .

Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167,748 –751.http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp .2010.09091379 Kasch, K. L., Rottenberg, J., Arnow, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2002).

Behavioral activation and inhibition systems and the severity and course of depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111,589 –597.http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.4.589 Keightley, M. L., Seminowicz, D. A., Bagby, R. M., Costa, P. T., Fossati, P., & Mayberg, H. S. (2003). Personality influences limbic-cortical interactions during sad mood induction.NeuroImage, 20,2031–2039.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.022 Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychi- atric and substance use disorders in men and women.Archives of General Psychiatry, 60,929 –937.http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60 .9.929 Kennedy, S. J., Rapee, R. M., & Edwards, S. L. (2009). A selective intervention program for inhibited preschool-aged children of parents with an anxiety disorder: Effects on current anxiety disorders and temperament.Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48,602– 609.http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181 9f6fa9 Khan, A. A., Jacobson, K. C., Gardner, C. O., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2005). Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric dis- orders.The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186,190 –196.http://dx.doi .org/10.1192/bjp.186.3.190 Kring, A. M., Persons, J. B., & Thomas, C. (2007). Changes in affect during treatment for depression and anxiety.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45,1753–1764.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.02.001 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 196 SAUER-ZAVALA, WILNER, AND BARLOW Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathol- ogy.Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2,111–133.http://dx.doi .org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095213 Lahey, B. B. (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism.American Psychologist, 64,241–256.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015309 Lanius, R. A., Frewen, P. A., Vermetten, E., & Yehuda, R. (2010). Fear conditioning and early life vulnerabilities: Two distinct pathways of emotional dysregulation and brain dysfunction in PTSD.European Journal of Psychotraumatology. Advance online publication.http://dx .doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v1i0.5467 McCabe, C., Mishor, Z., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2010). Diminished neural processing of aversive and rewarding stimuli during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment.Biological Psychiatry, 67,439 – 445.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.001 Meyer, B., Johnson, S. L., & Winters, R. (2001). Responsiveness to threat and incentive in Bipolar Disorder: Relations of the BIS/BAS scales with symptoms.Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 133–143.http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1010929402770 Moehler, E., Kagan, J., Oelkers-Ax, R., Brunner, R., Poustka, L., Haffner, J., & Resch, F. (2008). Infant predictors of behavioural inhibition.

British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26,145–150.http://dx .doi.org/10.1348/026151007X206767 Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A., III. (2003). Modeling intraindividual change in personality traits: Findings from the normative aging study.The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58,p153–p165.http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.3.P153 Murphy, S. E., Yiend, J., Lester, K. J., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J.

(2009). Short-term serotonergic but not noradrenergic antidepressant administration reduces attentional vigilance to threat in healthy volun- teers.International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 12,169 – 179.http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1461145708009164 Naragon-Gainey, K., Gallagher, M. W., & Brown, T. A. (2013). Stable “trait” variance of temperament as a predictor of the temporal course of depression and social phobia.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 611– 623.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032997 Payne, L. A., Ellard, K. K., Farchione, T. J., Fairholme, C. P., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). Emotional disorders: A unified transdiagnostic protocol. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.),Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual(5th ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Quilty, L. C., Meusel, L. A., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). Neuroticism as a mediator of treatment response to SSRIs in major depressive disorder.

Journal of Affective Disorders, 111,67–73.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j .jad.2008.02.006 Rapee, R. M., Kennedy, S., Ingram, M., Edwards, S., & Sweeney, L.

(2005). Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhib- ited preschool children.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73,488 – 497.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.488 Rapee, R. M., Kennedy, S. J., Ingram, M., Edwards, S. L., & Sweeney, L.

(2010). Altering the trajectory of anxiety in at-risk young children.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167,1518 –1525.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1176/appi.ajp.2010.09111619 Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adult- hood.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17,31–35.http://dx .doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta- analysis of longitudinal studies.Psychological Bulletin, 132,1–25.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1 Rosen, J. B., & Schulkin, J. (1998). From normal fear to pathological anxiety.Psychological Review, 105,325–350.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0033-295X.105.2.325 Sauer-Zavala, S., & Barlow, D. H. (2014). The case for borderline person- ality disorder as an emotional disorder: Implications for Treatment.Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 21,118 –138.http://dx.doi .org/10.1111/cpsp.12063 Sauer-Zavala, S., Bentley, K. H., & Wilner, J. G. (2016). Transdiagnostic treatment of borderline personality disorder and comorbid disorders: A clinical replication series.Journal of Personality Disorders, 30,35–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2015_29_179 Sauer-Zavala, S., Boswell, J. F., Gallagher, M. W., Bentley, K. H., Ametaj, A., & Barlow, D. H. (2012). The role of negative affectivity and negative reactivity to emotions in predicting outcomes in the unified protocol for the transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50,551– 557.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.05.005 Segal, Z., Teasdale, J., & Williams, M. (2002).Mindfulness-Based cogni- tive therapy for depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Shipley, B. A., Weiss, A., Der, G., Taylor, M. D., & Deary, I. J. (2007).

Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: A 21-year prospective cohort study.Psychosomatic Medicine, 69,923–931.http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815abf83 Small, B. J., Hertzog, C., Hultsch, D. F., & Dixon, R. A. (2003). Stability and change in adult personality over 6 years: Findings from the Victoria Longitudinal Study.The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psycholog- ical Sciences and Social Sciences, 58,P166 –P176.http://dx.doi.org/10 .1093/geronb/58.3.P166 Smith, T. W., & MacKenzie, J. (2006). Personality and risk of physical illness.Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2,435– 467.http://dx.doi .org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095257 Soskin, D. P., Carl, J. R., Alpert, J., & Fava, M. (2012). Antidepressant effects on emotional temperament: Toward a biobehavioral research paradigm for major depressive disorder.CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 18,441– 451.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-5949.2012.00318.x Spijker, J., de Graaf, R., Oldehinkel, A. J., Nolen, W. A., & Ormel, J. (2007).

Are the vulnerability effects of personality and psychosocial functioning on depression accounted for by subthreshold symptoms?Depression and Anx- iety, 24,472– 478.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.20252 Stein, M. B., Simmons, A. N., Feinstein, J. S., & Paulus, M. P. (2007).

Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164,318 – 327.http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.318 Suls, J., & Bunde, J. (2005). Anger, anxiety, and depression as risk factors for cardiovascular disease: The problems and implications of overlap- ping affective dispositions.Psychological Bulletin, 131,260 –300.http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.260 Tang, A. C., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., Romeo, R. D., & McEwen, B. S.

(2012). Reducing behavioral inhibition to novelty via systematic neo- natal novelty exposure: The influence of maternal hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal regulation.Biological Psychiatry, 72,150 –156.http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.021 Tang, T. Z., DeRubeis, R. J., Hollon, S. D., Amsterdam, J., Shelton, R., & Schalet, B. (2009). Personality change during depression treatment: A placebo-controlled trial.Archives of General Psychiatry, 66,1322–1330.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.166 Tomarken, A. J., Dichter, G. S., Freid, C., Addington, S., & Shelton, R. C.

(2004). Assessing the effects of bupropion SR on mood dimensions of depression.Journal of Affective Disorders, 78,235–241.http://dx.doi .org/10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00306-3 Warner, M. B., Morey, L. C., Finch, J. F., Gunderson, J. G., Skodol, A. E., Sanislow, C. A.,...Grilo, C. M. (2004). The longitudinal relationship of personality traits and disorders.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113,217–227.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.113.2.217 Weinstock, L. M., & Whisman, M. A. (2006). Neuroticism as a com- mon feature of the depressive and anxiety disorders: A test of the revised integrative hierarchical model in a national sample.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115,68 –74.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0021-843X.115.1.68 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 197 TREATMENT OF NEUROTICISM Westlye, L. T., Bjørnebekk, A., Grydeland, H., Fjell, A. M., & Walhovd, K. B. (2011). Linking an anxiety-related personality trait to brain white matter microstructure: Diffusion tensor imaging and harm avoidance.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 68,369 –377.http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archgenpsychiatry.2011.24 Widiger, T. A., & Smith, G. T. (2008).Personality and psychopathology.

Handbook of personality: Theory and research(3rd ed., pp. 743–769).

New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Plate tectonics in the classification of personality disorder: Shifting to a dimensional model.AmericanPsychologist, 62,71– 83.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62 .2.71 Widiger, T. A., Verheul, R., & van den Brink, W. (1999). Personality and psychopathology. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.),Handbook of personality: Theory and research(2nd ed., pp. 347–366). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Dubo, E. D., Sickel, A. E., Trikha, A., Levin, A., & Reynolds, V. (1998). Axis II comorbidity of borderline personality disorder.Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39,296 –302.http://dx .doi.org/10.1016/S0010-440X(98)90038-4 Members of Underrepresented Groups:

Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate more in this process.

If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write APA Journals at [email protected].

Please note the following important points:

•To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective review.

•To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical journals that are most central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission within the context of existing research.

•To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information.

Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you are interested in, and describe your area of expertise. Be as specific as possible. For example, “social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude change” as well.

•Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1– 4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript thoroughly.

APA now has an online video course that provides guidance in reviewing manuscripts. To learn more about the course and to access the video, visit http://www.apa.org/pubs/authors/review- manuscript-ce-video.aspx. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 198 SAUER-ZAVALA, WILNER, AND BARLOW