Online Wellness Course: Discussion Board Assignment - Four parts

South African Journal of Psychology 43(2) 147 –155 © The Author(s) 2013 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/0081246313484238 sap.sagepub.com Wellness without fairness: The missing link in psychology Isaac Prilleltensky Abstract To promote human welfare, psychologists must advance two important goals\ : wellness and fairness. Hitherto, research on wellness or well-being has discovered connections among overall satisfaction with life and important facets of life, such as relationshi\ ps, income, and physical health, but the connections among various types of wellness and specific\ aspects of fairness remain obscure. Research on justice in psychology, in turn, has focused \ largely on the impact of distributive, procedural, and interactional justice on job performance a\ nd not so much on wellness outcomes. I argue that psychologists must explore in depth the associati\ on among various types of wellness, such as interpersonal, occupational, physical, and psycholo\ gical, and diverse kinds of fairness, such as distributive, procedural, interpersonal, cultural, dev\ elopmental, retributive, and intrapersonal. The pursuit of wellness without fairness will not yield t\ he outcomes individuals and communities need. We must make more explicit the relationship betwee\ n justice or injustice and flourishing in life.

Keywords Ethics, fairness, justice, well-being, wellness Introduction Wellness, or well-being, has been correlated in psychology with many fact\ ors, such as health, rela - tionships, income, housing, free time, religion, and transportation (Bu\ ettner, 2008, 2010; Chmiel, Brunner, Martin, & Schalke, 2012; Cohen, 1999; Nieboer, Lindenberg, Boomsma, & Van Bruggen, 2005; Rath & Harter, 2010), to name but a few; yet, the association between wellness and f\ airness has been largely neglected from our discipline (Prilleltensky, 2012). Other fields such as public health (De Vogli, Ferrie, Chandola, Kivimäki, & Marmot, 2007; Donohoe, 2013; Frei\ dl, Fazekas, Raml, Pretis, & Feistritzer, 2007; Levy & Sidel, 2006), human resources (Bernhard-Oettel, De Cuyper, Schreurs, & De Witte, 2011), political science (Mutz & Mondak, 1997; Sun & Xiao, University of Miami, USA Corresponding author:Isaac Prilleltensky, School of Education and Human Development, Universi\ ty of Miami, P.O. Box 248065, Coral Gables, FL 33124, USA. Email: [email protected] 484238 SAP 43 2 10.1177/0081246313484238 South African Journal of PsychologyPrilleltensky 2013 State of the Science Editorial 148 South African Journal of Psychology 43(2) 2012), and philosophy (Segev, 2006; Sen, 2009) have surely dealt with the wellness–fairness nexus, but the psychological literature reveals few studies that directl\ y address the connection between well-being and justice. Some researchers have analyzed the relationship between fairness and well-being in particular settings, such as the family (Grote, Clark\ , & Moore, 2004; Ullmann- Margalit, 2006), schools (Schmader, Major, & Gramzow, 2001), and the workplace (Fujishiro, 2005; Kivimäki et al., 2005), but we are hard pressed to find any studies that examine co\ mprehen - sively the connection between the many facets of wellness with the many \ types of fairness (Miller, 2001). A notable exception, not yet available in English, is the compilation in \ Italian by Santinello and Vieno (2011), which contains chapters on the impact of injustice on well-being in\ schools, work, relationships, and groups. To be sure, there is scholarship exploring connections among constructs a\ ssociated with fairness and wellness, such as Fiske’s (2011) work on envy and scorn and their deleterious repercussions for the envious, the envied, the scornful, and the scorned; Miller’s (2001) work on injustice and disrespect and their negative impact on self-image; research on microagg\ ressions and their impact on mental health (Blume, Lovato, Thyken, & Denny, 2012; Torres, Driscoll, & Burrow, 2010); and effects of discrimination on stress (Fuller-Rowell, Evans, & Ong, 2012; Lee & Ahn, 2012; Pascoe & Smart Richman, 2009); or child abuse, an obvious form of injustice wi\ th multiple negative psy - chological outcomes (Prilleltensky, Nelson, & Peirson, 2001a); but in my view, we need to estab - lish a more clear line of research and action dealing specifically with \ the complex relationship among various domains of wellness and diverse types of fairness. I want \ to make this case by defining wellness and its many facets, fairness, and its various types; \ by discussing the presumed connections among the two constructs; and by proposing next steps. To prevent confusion, I will use wellness and well-being interchangeably, and will do the same for fairness and justice.

What is wellness?

Well-being is both a positive and desirable state of affairs with life as a whole and with specific domains of life, such as health, economic situation, and relationships (\ Chmiel et al., 2012; Diener, Helliwell, & Kahneman, 2010; Huppert & Linley, 2011a, 2011b; Prilleltensky & Prilleltensky, 2006). Significant and consistent correlations have been found among perceptions of satisfaction with life as a whole and satisfaction with specific facets of life (Cohen, 1999; Cummins, Mellor, Stokes, & Lau, 2010; Nieboer et al., 2005; Pavot & Diener, 2008). Rath and Harter (2010) have recently summarized results from data coll\ ected by the Gallup Corporation around the world and concluded that five factors constitute \ the key elements of well- being: career, social relationships, community, health, and finances. In an effort to synthesize the central domains of well-being, my research team integrated the vast literature of facets of well- being and determined that there are six key domains summarized in the ac\ ronym I COPPE, which stands for Interpersonal, Community, Occupational, Psychological, Physical, and Economic well- being. Our team assessed the correlations among six specific domains and\ overall wellness and found meaningful, positive, and high correlations among each domain and \ satisfaction with life as a whole (Prilleltensky et al., 2013). This line of research explores subjective well-being or the perception of satisfaction with life by research participants. In addition to subjective evaluations of well-being, there are obviously objective assessments of wellness, such as income, density of social networks, crime rates, or si\ gns of physical health, including body temperature, blood pressure, and cholesterol levels. Both\ kinds of measures, objec - tive and subjective, are important and complementary, and the neglect of either can lead to incom - prehensible paradoxes, such as the ones experienced in Colombia and Mexi\ co in recent times. Prilleltensky 149 In the last decade of the past and the first decade of the present century, Colombians and Mexicans reported, respectively, the highest level of satisfaction in the world. This was at the time when both countries reported extreme levels of random violence, kidnappings, and k\ illings. Had we looked only at their subjective perceptions of well-being, we would have obtain\ ed a very incomplete pic - ture of happiness in their countries. Similarly, had we looked only at crime statistics, we would have predicted that their populations would be in a constant state of de\ pression. The seeming para - dox that crime and random violence can co-exist is explained by the surge of democracy in these countries during the periods in question, and by the traditional emphasi\ s these countries put on family, friendships, and overall close relationships (Graham, 2009; Inglehart\ , 2010; Inglehart, Foa, Peterson, & Welzel, 2008). These protective mechanisms account for the well-being of the popula - tion and compensate for the dismal lack of security people experience in\ these countries. A com - prehensive portrayal of well-being in individuals and communities requir\ es objective and subjective appraisals. Focusing on the latter, at the expense of the former, has led psychologists like Seligman (2002) to erroneous conclusions, whereby he claimed that income and ed\ ucation do not matter for well-being, whereas there is ample evidence that people with both higher\ education and income experience fewer psychosocial problems and higher levels of self-esteem \ and self-efficacy (Donohoe, 2013; Freidl et al., 2007; Fuller-Rowell et al., 2012; Levy & Sidel, 2006; Pascoe & Smart Richman, 2009; Prilleltensky, 2012; Prilleltensky & Prilleltensky, 2006; Rath & Harter, 2010; Wilkinson & Pickett, 2009). A preoccupation with intra-psychic dynamics and a misplaced emphasis on r\ esilience have led researchers such as Seligman (2002) to ignore contextual factors in we\ ll-being, such as income, education, and opportunities in life (Ehrenreich, 2009). While the human mind has great capacity for adaptation, and the human spirit has great endurance, it is a mistake to presume that most indi - viduals can overcome adversity unscathed, or that external factors can b\ e overcome by internal pirouettes of the mind. There is no question that some people, endowed with intelligence and empathy can, with appropriate supports, overcome adversity, but they remain a minority (Prilleltensky, Nelson, & Peirson, 2001b). For the vast majority who face oppression \ and injustice, life becomes a constant struggle (Nelson & Prilleltensky, 2010), which is why it is so surprising that psychologists have not explored in depth yet the connection between\ fairness and wellness. It is encouraging to see that some psychologists have begun to make the emp\ irical connection between wellness and fairness more explicit (Grote et al., 2004; Miller, 2001; Santinello & Vieno, 2011; Schmader et al., 2001), but much remains to be done to understand the reciprocal na\ ture of types of wellness and types of fairness.

Why is wellness important?

Well-being has not only intrinsic but extrinsic value as well. People who\ report high levels of well- being experience fewer psychological and physical problems, better relat\ ionships, live longer, are more productive at work, miss fewer days of work, and have reduced risk \ of suicide (Keyes, 2005, 2007; Keyes, Dhingra, & Simoes, 2010; Keyes et al., 2012; Keyes & Grzywacz, 2005; Keyes & Simoes, 2012). The development of the two-factor model of mental health has generated g\ reat interest in well- being among clinicians and researchers. They see well-being as a viable route to complete mental health. The two-factor model demonstrates that complete mental health depends on\ the reduction of mental illness and the enhancement of well-being (Keyes, 2005, 2007). Much effort has been invested in the former but definitely not enough in the latter. Keyes (2007) has made a strong case for balancing the two paradigms. It is no longer acceptable to promote c\ omplete mental health by 150 South African Journal of Psychology 43(2) reducing mental illness alone. We must, concurrently, promote well-being. Complete mental health requires that we focus more attention on understanding and promoting wel\ l-being. Keyes (2005, 2007) has empirically shown that fostering well-being can reduce the le\ ngth, severity, and preva - lence of mental illness. Hitherto, we have established that wellness has many facets, that it can\ be evaluated through objective and subjective means, that it can promote complete mental heal\ th, and that it is very likely affected by different types of fairness. There is emerging evidence that fairness, or lack thereof, affect levels of wellness (De la Sablonnière, Auger, Sadykova, & Taylor, 2010; Fiske, 2011; Grote et al., 2004; Miller, 2001; Santinello & Vieno, 2011; Schmader et al., 2001). To under - stand this connection better, and to offer a research and action agenda, we need to explore more in depth the various types of fairness that can affect wellness.

What is fairness?

Following Rawls (2001), I will use fairness as synonymous with justice\ . There are two main types of fairness: distributive and procedural (Colquitt, LePine, Piccolo, Za\ pata, & Rich, 2012; Tornblom & Vermunt, 2007). The former refers to outcomes. The latter refers to process.

Distributive justice pertains to the fair and equitable distribution of \ resources, gains and pains, benefits, and obligations in society. When people talk about social justice, they talk mostly about distributive justice. How to allocate goods such as health care, and burdens such as taxes, are central concerns of distributive justice. People and systems engage in d\ istributive dealings all the time. It is not just structures like government that care about distribu\ tion, but groups, families, and dyads as well (Elster, 1992, 1995). From who does the dishes, to who pays the bills, distributive justice questions are ubiquitous (Miller, 2001). Getting your fair share, carrying your load, and getting what you deserve are all questions of distributive justice. How \ to apportion to each his or her due has concerned moral philosophers and social scientists for ages \ (Corning, 2011; Elster, 1992, 1995; Sandel, 2009). Whereas some favor need, and others merit, several thinkers favor a contextual approach whereby the criterion chosen corresponds to the circ\ umstances of the case.

In cases of inequality, the contextualists argue, need should be preferred to level the playing field.

In cases of equality, where all start life with similar endowments and opportunities, merit should be chosen to reward effort (Corning, 2011; Deutsch, 1975; Facione, Scherer, & Attig, 1978).

Psychologists have noted that people are sensitive to distributional fai\ rness, but they have studied mainly their cognitive and decision-making processes (Miller, 2001), or job performance (Colquitt et al., 2012) associated with fairness. The connections among various types of fairness and vari - ous types of wellness remain a fertile territory to explore. The second main type of fairness is procedural justice. As opposed to distributive justice, which is concerned with the what, procedural is concerned with the how. Human beings have a strong need to participate in processes and decisions affecting their lives. In the literature, this is called voice.

Lack of voice renders individuals angry and frustrated, with a great sense of injustice. In the organi - zational development literature, procedural justice figures prominently \ as a correlate of work satis - faction and productivity (Colquitt et al., 2012; Miller, 2001; Tornblom & Vermunt, 2007). Recent work by the Whitehall study team in England discovered strong associations between feeling respected at work and physical and mental health outcomes (De Vogli et al., 2007; Elovainio, Kivimäki, & Vahtera, 2002; Kivimäki et al., 2005). The higher the level of respect experienced at work, the better the levels of worker well-being. Their investigations represent the most explicit empirical statement yet about the connections among one type of justice—\ procedural—and spe - cific types of well-being—physical and emotional. Prilleltensky 151 In addition to the two main types of fairness—distributive and procedural—there are a number of subtypes that are more contextually based. In organizations, researchers have identified infor - mational justice (Colquitt, 2001; Greenberg & Colquitt, 2005), which refers to access to knowl - edge and transparency. In relationships, there is interpersonal justice, which is a combinati\ on of distributive and procedural justice: getting your fair share and having a voice in the relationship (Grote et al., 2004; Hatfield, Rapson, & Aumer-Ryan, 2008; Rayner, 1999; Ullmann-Margalit, 2006). In cultural relations, there is cultural justice, or the fair tr\ eatment of minorities or ethnic groups. In the legal system, there is retributive justice. Across generations, there is developmental justice or injustice. This is evident in cases of child or elder abuse (Prilleltensky, 2012). While these types of fairness are played out in relationships between two or m\ ore individuals, I claim that people can also be fair or unfair to themselves, leading me to think tha\ t there must be intrapersonal justice or injustice. This is obvious in cases of self-injurious behavior at one extreme (eat\ ing disor - ders, self-mutilation, or suicide) or self-deprecating talk at the othe\ r. We can be unfair to ourselves.

This type of injustice leads me to think that we can probably invoke psy\ chological injustice. When we disrespect ourselves or others, we engage in some kind of psychologic\ al lack of fairness. So far, we have seen that there are two main types and a few subtypes of fairn\ ess. In the next section, I will propose that we study the various kinds of fairness acro\ ss contexts, for each situation can involve more than one type of justice.

Why is fairness important?

Justice impacts our lives in multiple planes: From the relationship we have with ourselves, to the relationship we have with family, colleagues, citizens, and our government (Elster, 1992, 1995; Miller, 2001). In my view, we can think of each level of interaction—from the intrapersonal to\ the interpersonal to the systemic—as arenas of fairness. At each level of interaction, there can be one or multiple types of fairness at play. Consider interpersonal relationships. In this arena, there are distribut\ ive, procedural, informa - tional, and psychological types of justice at play. Do I get my fair share of resources? Is my voice taken into account when we make decisions? Does my partner share with me\ essential information about finances? Am I respected in the relationship? Similar questions arise in work sett\ ings: Am I compensated fairly for my work? Does the boss care about what I have to \ say? Do I feel respected by my colleagues? and Do they share with me important and timely informa\ tion? Even in conver - sations we have with ourselves there can be more than one type of justic\ e involved: Do I claim my legitimate due in the world? Do I treat myself with respect or do I put \ myself down? The explora - tion of types of justice across levels of human experience is sure to deliver interesting and useful insights about the connection between fairness and wellness. What is more, this approach can shed light on the complementary nature o\ f various types of fairness. Take the case of educational underachievement by ethnic minorities. If we\ tackle this issue from a distributive justice perspective, we would want to know the impact of lack of resources and opportunities on the poor educational attainment of the group in que\ stion. However, if we stay within distributive justice and concentrate only on the problems and def\ icits of a group due to lack of resources, we run the risk of stigmatizing it. This is why we need to invoke cultural justice, which is about the fair treatment of diverse groups. Within minority groups, there are certain to be role models who succeed against many odds. It is a matter of cultural ju\ stice to recognize the strengths of minorities and celebrate them. Individuals within the minor\ ity group who succeed despite challenges can feel justifiably stigmatized if their assets are \ not acknowledged. There is literature in psychology dealing with the negative effects of being ignored while succeeding 152 South African Journal of Psychology 43(2) (Gable, Gonzaga, & Strachman, 2006), just as there is literature on th\ e tyranny of poor expecta - tions of minorities, conceptualized as a form of microaggression (Torres et al., 2010). Failing to celebrate somebody’s success can amount to an injustice of strengths. People need to be recognized for their achievements. If we were to focus only on the distributive injustices and the underach\ ievement of an ethnic group, without acknowledging that some individuals within said group ove\ rcome adversity and succeed, we would have engaged also in cultural injustice, because we wo\ uld have stereotyped an entire group. Moreover, we would have failed to acknowledge their effort, which is a form of dis - respect. We can see, through this example, interactions among diverse types of jus\ tice, and various forms of well-being. Complete well-being demands that we pay complementary attention to what is lacking in people’s lives—resources and access to good schools—with what is present—\ talent and success. Focusing on the former without the latter can stigmatize cu\ ltural subgroups. This analysis also reveals that people can experience one type of justice (e\ .g., distributive), but not oth - ers (psychological or procedural), leading to incomplete states of fai\ rness and wellness.

Future steps Psychologists have explored various facets of well-being (Pavot & Diene\ r, 2008), but have largely neglected the role of fairness on wellness. Analogously, psychologists have studied various aspects of fairness, such as reactions to distributive and procedural justice and injustice (Miller, 2001; Tornblom & Vermunt, 2007), but have yet to explore the impact of these dynamics on wellness.

With distinct exceptions, such as the work of Santinello and Vieno (2011), much conceptual and empirical work needs to be done to distill the connections among wellnes\ s and fairness. Based on the preceding discussion, I would suggest that we (1) create a conceptual map to link various types of fairness with diverse aspects of wellness; (2) consid\ er the impact of various types of fairness on various domains of wellness; (3) expand our studies of justice beyond distributive and procedural to include developmental, cultural, intrapersonal, and ps\ ychological; (4) study the presence of various types of fairness across diverse contexts: intrapers\ onal, interpersonal, organi - zational, and systemic; and (5) examine the complementary nature of va\ rious types of fairness, such as cultural with distributive, to achieve full wellness, or flouris\ hing. Research along these lines can do much to propel specific action on fairness to improve and e\ nhance wellness, for indi - viduals and communities alike. This is the promise of psychology to the world, and this is what we need to do.

Declaration of conflicting interests The author declares that he does not have any conflict of interest.

Funding This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References Bernhard-Oettel, C., De Cuyper, N., Schreurs, B., & De Witte, H. (2011)\ . Linking job insecurity to well-being and organizational attitudes in Belgian workers: The role of \ security expectations and fairness. The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 22 , 1866–1886. doi: 10.1080/09585192.2011.573967 Prilleltensky 153 Blume, A. W., Lovato, L. V., Thyken, B. N., & Denny, N. (2012). The re\ lationship of microaggressions with alcohol use and anxiety among ethnic minority college students in a\ historically White institution.

Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology , 18, 45–54. Buettner, D. (2008). The blue zones: Lessons for living longer from the people who’ve lived the longest . Washington, DC: National Geographic. Buettner, D. (2010). Thrive . Washington, DC: National Geographic. Chmiel, M., Brunner, M., Martin, R., & Schalke, D. (2012). Revisiting \ the structure of subjective well-being in middle-aged adults. Social Indicators Research, 106 , 109–116. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9796-7 Cohen, E. H. (1999). A facet theory approach to examining overall and \ life facet satisfaction relationships. Social Indicators Research , 51, 223–237. Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justic\ e: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology , 86, 386–400. Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., Piccolo, R. F., Zapata, C. P., & Rich, B\ . L. (2012). Explaining the justice- performance relationship: Trust as exchange deepener or trust as uncerta\ inty reducer? Journal of Applied Psychology, 97 , 1–15. Corning, P. (2011). The fair society: The science of human nature and the pursuit of social \ justice . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Cummins, R. A., Mellor, D., Stokes, M. A., & Lau, A. L. D. (2010). Measures of subjective well-being. In E. Mpofu & T. Oakland (Eds.), Rehabilitation and health assessment: Applying ICF guidelines (pp. 409–426). New York, NY: Springer. De la Sablonnière, R., Auger, É., Sadykova, N., & Taylor, D. M. (2010). When the “We” impacts how “I” feel about myself: Effect of temporal collective relative deprivation on\ personal well-being in the context of dramatic social change in Kyrgyzstan. European Psychologist , 15, 271–282. Deutsch, M. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice. Journal of Social Issues , 31(3), 137–149. De Vogli, R., Ferrie, J., Chandola, T., Kivimäki, M., & Marmot, M. (\ 2007). Unfairness and health: Evidence from the Whitehall II study. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health , 61, 513–518. Diener, E., Helliwell, J., & Kahneman, D. (Ed.). (2010). International differences in well-being . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Donohoe M. (Ed.). (2013). Public health and social justice . San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Ehrenreich, B. (2009). Bright sided: How the relentless promotion of positive thinking has unde\ rmined America . New York, NY: Holt. Elovainio, M., Kivimäki, M., & Vahtera, J. (2002). Organizational justice: Evidence of a new psychosocial predictor of health. American Journal of Public Health , 92, 105–108. Elster, J. (1992). Local justice: How institutions allocate scarce goods and necessary burd\ ens . New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. Elster J. (Ed.). (1995). Local justice in America . New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. Facione, P. A., Scherer, D., & Attig, T. (1978). Values and society: An introduction to ethics and social phi - losophy . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Fiske, S. (2011). Envy up, scorn down: How status divides us . New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. Freidl, W., Fazekas, C., Raml, R., & Pretis, Feistritzer, G. (2007). P\ erceived social justice, long-term unem - ployment and health: A survey among marginalized groups in Austria. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 42 , 547–553. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0207-y Fujishiro, K. (2005). Fairness at work: Its impacts on employee well-being (Unpublished doctoral disserta - tion). The Ohio State University, Columbus. Fuller-Rowell, T. E., Evans, G. W., & Ong, A. D. (2012). Poverty and h\ ealth: The mediating role of perceived discrimination. Psychological Science, 23 , 734–739. doi: 10.1177/0956797612439720 Gable, S., Gonzaga, G., & Strachman, A. (2006). Will you be there for \ me when things go right? Supportive responses to positive events disclosures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 90, 904–917. Graham, C. (2009). Happiness around the world: The paradox of happy peasants and miserable \ millionaires . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 154 South African Journal of Psychology 43(2) Greenberg J. & J. Colquitt (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of organizational justice . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Grote, N. K., Clark, M. S., & Moore, A. (2004). Perceptions of injusti\ ce in family work: The role of psycho - logical distress. Journal of Family Psychology , 18, 480–492. Hatfield, E., Rapson, R., & Aumer-Ryan, K. (2008). Social justice in l\ ove relationships: Recent develop - ments. Social Justice Research , 21, 413–431. Huppert, F. A., & Linley, P. A. (2011a). Happiness and well-being: Critical concepts in psychology, Vol. III: Causes and correlates of happiness and well-being: What makes us happy . New York, NY: Routledge. Huppert, F. A., & Linley, P. A. (2011b). Happiness and well-being: Critical concepts in psychology, Vol. IV: The impacts and enhancement of happiness and well-being: Consequence\ s, interventions and public policy . New York, NY: Routledge. Inglehart, R. (2010). Faith and freedom: Traditional and modern ways to happiness. In E. Diener, J. Helliwell, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp. 351–397). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Inglehart, R., Foa, R., Peterson, C., & Welzel, C. (2008). Development, freedom, and rising happiness. Perspectives on Psychological Science , 3, 264–285. Keyes, C. (2005). Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating a\ xioms of the complete state model of health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology , 73, 539–548. Keyes, C. (2007). Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishin\ g. American Psychologist , 62, 95–108. Keyes, C., Dhingra, S., & Simoes, E. (2010). Change in level of positi\ ve mental health as a predictor of future risk of mental illness. American Journal of Public Health , 100 , 2366–2371. Keyes, C., Eisenberg, D., Perry, G., Dube, S., Kroenke, K., & Dhingra, S\ . (2012). The relationship of level of positive mental health with current mental disorders in predicting suicidal behavior and academic impair - ment in college students. Journal of American College Health , 60, 126–132. Keyes, C., & Grzywacz, J. (2005). Health as a complete state: The added value in work performance and healthcare costs. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine , 47, 523–532. Keyes, C., & Simoes, E. (2012). To flourish or not: Positive mental he\ alth and all-cause mortality. American Journal of Public Health , 102 , 2164–2172. Kivimäki, M., Ferrie, J. E., Brunner, E., Head, J., Shipley, M., Vaht\ era, J., & Marmot, M. (2005). Justice at work and reduced risk of coronary heart disease among employees. Archives of Internal Medicine , 165 , 2245–2251. Lee, D. L., & Ahn, S. (2012). Discrimination against Latina/os: A meta\ -analysis of individual-level resources and outcomes. The Counseling Psychologist, 40 , 28–65. doi: 10.1177/0011000011403326 Levy B. & Sidel V. (Ed.). (2006). Social injustice and public health . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Miller, D. T. (2001). Disrespect and the experience if injustice. Annual Review of Psychology , 52, 527–553. Mutz, D. C., & Mondak, J. J. (1997). Dimensions of sociotropic behavio\ r: Group-based judgments of fairness and well-being. American Journal of Political Science , 41, 284–308. Nelson, G. B., & Prilleltensky, I. (2010). Community psychology: In pursuit of liberation and well-being (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan. Nieboer, A., Lindenberg, S., Boomsma, A., & Van Bruggen, A. C. (2005).\ Dimensions of well-being and their measurement—The SPF-IL Scale. Social Indicators Research , 73, 313–353. Pascoe, E. A., & Smart Richman, L. S. (2009). Perceived discrimination\ and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin , 135 , 531–554. Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (2008). The Satisfaction With Life Scale and t\ he emerging construct of life satisfac - tion. The Journal of Positive Psychology , 3, 137–152. Prilleltensky, I. (2012). Wellness as fairness. American Journal of Community Psychology , 49, 1–21. Prilleltensky, I., Dietz, S., Prilleltensky, O., Rubenstein, C., Myers, \ N., Jing, Y., & McMahon, A. (2013). Assessing multidimensional well-being: Development and validation of the\ I COPPE Scale . Unpublished manuscript. Prilleltensky, I., Nelson, G., & Peirson, L. (2001a). Promoting family wellness and preventing child maltreat - ment: Fundamentals for thinking and action . Toronto, ON, Canada: University of Toronto Press. Prilleltensky 155 Prilleltensky, I., Nelson, G., & Peirson, L. (2001b). The role of powe\ r and control in children’s lives: An eco - logical analysis of pathways towards wellness, resilience, and problems. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology , 11, 143–158. Prilleltensky, I., & Prilleltensky, O. (2006). Promoting well-being: Linking personal, organizational, and community change . Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Rath, T., & Harter, J. (2010). Well-being: The five essential elements . New York, NY: Gallup Press. Rawls, J. (2001). Justice as fairness . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Rayner, E. (1999). Some functions of being fair and just-or not-in cli\ nical psychoanalysis. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis , 80, 477–492. Sandel, M. (2009). Justice: What’s the right thing to do . New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Santinello, M., & Vieno, A. (Eds.). (2011). Non é giusto. Psicologia dell’ingiustizia sociale [It’s not fair. The psychology of social injustice]. Napoli, Italy: Liguori Editore. Schmader, T., Major, B., & Gramzow, R. H. (2001). Coping with ethnic s\ tereotypes in the academic domain: Perceived injustice and psychological disengagement. Journal of Social Issues , 57, 93–111. Segev, R. (2006). Well-being and fairness. Philosophical Studies, 131 , 369–391. doi: 10.1007/s11098-004- 7484-z Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your p\ otential for lasting fulfillment . New York, NY: Free Press. Sen, A. (2009). The idea of justice . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Sun, F., & Xiao, J. J. (2012). Perceived social policy fairness and su\ bjective wellbeing: Evidence from China. Social Indicators Research , 107 , 171–186. Tornblom, K., & Vermunt, R. (Eds.). (2007). Distributive and procedural justice: Research and social appli - cations . Burlington, VT: Ashgate. Torres, L., Driscoll, M. W., & Burrow, A. L. (2010). Racial microaggre\ ssions and psychological functioning among highly achieving African-Americans: A mixed-methods approach. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology , 29, 1074–1099. Ullmann-Margalit, E. (2006). Family fairness. Social Research , 73, 575–598. Wilkinson, R., & Pickett, K. (2009). The spirit level: Why more equal societies almost always do better . London, UK: Penguin Books.