write a summary

See d is c u ssio n s, s ta ts , a n d a u th or p ro fil e s f o r t h is p ub lic a tio n a t:

http :/ /w ww.r e se a rc h ga te .n et/ p ub lic a tio n /2 55635788

When A re R acia l D is p arit ie s

in E du ca tio n t h e R esu lt o f

Racia l D is crim in atio n ? A

So cia l S cie n ce P ers p ectiv e

AR TIC LE

in

TEA C H ER S C O LLE G E R EC O RD

· A U GU ST 2 003

Im pact F a cto r: 0 .7 9

·

DO I: 1 0.1 111/1 467-9 620.0 0277

CIT A TIO NS

47

DO W NLO AD S

647

VIE W S

124

1 A U TH O R:

Rosly n A rlin M ic k e ls o n

Univ e rs it y o f N orth C aro lin

58

PU BLIC ATIO NS

1,0 46

CIT A TIO NS

SEE P R O FIL E

Ava il a b le f r o m : R osly n A rlin M ic k e ls o n

Retr ie ve d o n : 0 8 S ep te m ber 2 015 When Are Racial Disparities in Education

the Result of Racial Discrimination? A

Social Science Perspective

ROSLYN ARLIN MICKELSON

University of North Carolina at Charlotte'

In this article I seek to answer the question, "When are racial disparities in education the result of racial discrimination?" To answer it I synthesize the social science research on racially correlated disparities in education. My review draws from thte sociology, anthropology, political science, psychology, history, and education literatures. I organize explanations into six categories: biological determinism, social structure, school organization and opportunities to learn, family background, culture, and the state. I arrive at three answers. The first is a definition: Racial discrimination in education arises from actions of institutions or individual state actors, their attitudes and ideologies, or processes that systematically treat students from different racial/ethnic groups disparately or inequitably. The second answer is that while distinguishing racial discrimination from disparities may be an interesting intellectual, legal, and statistical challenge, the conclusion probably is less meaningful than social scientists and policy makers might hope. The third answer follows from the first two. I propose the following reformulation of the original question: "When are racial disparities in education not due to discrimination?" I argue that the reformulated question is more likely to bring solutions to the race gap than the original one. Even if we conclude that discrimination does not cause racial disparities in education, we should not conclude that schools have no role in addressing them. If public schools do not address educational disparities, then who or what institution will?

Globalization, immigration, and the post-Fordist information-based econ- omy are transforming 21st century America. The hallmarks of globaliza- tion-the flow of capital, information, and people across political boundaries-are sure to enrich, challenge, and complicate contemporary society. Public education already is experiencing many of the challenges associated with these trends. Schools and communities are more ethnically and racially diverse than ever before. Racial and ethnic disparities in education are not new to America's schools. Recent demographic, political, and economic shifts have the potential to exacerbate existing inequalities.

Teachers College Record Volume 105, Number 6, August 2003, pp. 1052-1086 Copyright © by Teachers College, Columbia University 0161-4681 Wlhen Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1053

In addition, social class and gender divisions complicate the emerging racial

gaps in educational outcomes.

Identifying when disparities are the result of discrimination is difficult.

Historically, racial discrimination was integral to public education, the

intentional result of discriminatory laws and practices. Jim Grow education

was designed, implemented, and upheld by the state (Anderson, 1988;

Kluger, 1977; Walker, 1999; Walters, 2001; Watkins, 2001).2 Since the

middle of the last century, however, laws, court rulings, and policies of the

state-along with many heroic efforts by private citizens-have eliminated

the formal legal architecture of educational discrimination. During the

past three decades legal segregation has been outlawed, although de

facto resegregation is on the increase (Clodfelter, Ladd, & Vigdor, 2002;

Orfield & Gordon, 2001; Orfield & Yun, 1999; Yun & Reardon, 2002).

Importantly, literacy and median years of schooling are comparable

among blacks and whites, multicultural curricula are used widely; and

overtly racist material has been eliminated. Gamoran (2001) points out

that any comparison of American education today with that of 1901 reveals

overt racial discrimination and disparities in school outcomes have been

reduced dramatically. Nevertheless, racially correlated disparities continue.

This article has two tasks: first, to synthesize the extant social science

research on racially correlated disparities in education to better understand

their structural and cultural antecedents because doing so helps clarify

when such disparities are the results of discrimination and, second, to

identify the key points in students' educational trajectories at which

discrimination is likely to occur. My review of the social science literature

draws from the sociology, anthropology, political science, psychology,

history, and education literatures. Although I attend to student-level

factors, I heed Coleman's (1994) admonition to scholars and make the

explanatory focus of this article the social system, not the individual.

I begin by theorizing race and racial identity because they are central to

any discussion of discrimination in education and are crucial to the social

dynamics in racially plural societies like America. In the next section I

catalog current racial disparities in education and propose how we might

conceptualize when disparities are due to discrimination. In the third

section I synthesize the substantive and theoretical social science literature

accounting for racial disparities and discrimination in education. I conclude

with a series of answers to the question suggested in the title, what

constitutes racial discrimination in education? The final answer reconcep-

tualizes the core problem itself: Rather than considering when disparities

reflect discrimination, I propose that the more appropriate question is

"When are racial disparities in education not discrimination?" I announce

this reformulation now in the hope that it will guide the reading of

this article. 1054 Teachers College Record

CONSTRUCTING RACE AND ETHNICITY

Race and racial identity are central to this article, but these subjects are too immense to be fully theorized here. I consider race and ethnicity to be historically contingent social constructs intimately entwined with identity; they are meaningful and consequential in a sociopolitical and historical context (Waters, 1990). Recent theories of race and ethnicity, such as racial formation theory (Omi & Winant, 1986) and critical race theory (Delgado & Stefancic, 2001) require us to consider the process-oriented and relational character of racial meanings and identities.

RACE AND ETHNICITY

Social scientists generally agree that races are socially constructed in loose

relation to perceived phenotypical differences among humans. Ethnicity relates to national ancestry and signifies the cultural, linguistic, and

historical differences among groups. Key aspects of ethnicity are the beliefs on the part of people who identify with an ethnic group that they descend from common ancestors, share a common culture with coethnics, and choose to identify with that ethnic group (Waters, 1990).

A group's history of contact with the United States, the conditions of its incorporation into American society, and the contemporary politics of an ethnic group's country of origin influence the way an ethnic group constructs its identity, the nature of the education historically provided, and the barriers or bridges to education currently afforded its children. For example, the U.S. government extends special educational privileges to most Cuban American children because of their status as political refugees from a communist state. Haitians, however, are defined as economic refugees and thus do not enjoy these privileges (Schmidt, 2001; Van Hook, 2002).

RACIAL IDENTITY

Racial self-identity has two complementary aspects: how others identify a person's race and how the person constructs her or his own racial identity, in part as a reaction to others' behavior. Both aspects are relevant to racial discrimination in education. A student's racial or ethnic self-identity need not be consistent with others' construction of that identity. On the other hand, racial dynamics in the United States are concerned less with how an individual constructs his or her racial identity than with how others perceive, construct, and label it (Fordham, 1996; McCarthy & Crichlow, 1993). To capture the dynamic interplay between culture, structure, and agency in producing discrimination in education, one must consider the contributions of all three to racial identity. When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1055

O'Connor (2001, p. 159) observes that, to make sense of the ways that

culture, structure, and human agency affect achievement outcomes, we

must understand how people are positioned differently in the social world

and how in that world individuals have multiple social identities. She

observes that students experience their own structured and cultured social

identity both by interpreting and performing their identities (reflection)

and by registering and reacting to others' responses to them (refraction). To

paraphrase Cousins (2002), racial identity is not simply about being black,

biracial, white, Latino, Asian, or Native American in a white world but about

being so in a white, media- and technology-saturated, capitalist- and

information-driven globalizing world of relations between youths and

adults, boys and girls, and men and women.

LABELS

To truly fulfill the article's mission, would require me to include

comparative research on students who are Asian, Latino, biracial, and

Native American (including American Indians, Inuits, Hawaiians, and other

Pacific Islanders), as well as black and white. Even panethnic labels such as

Asian or Latino mask important ethnic distinctions within these larger

categories (Lopez & Espiritu, 1990). Any comprehensive treatment of

discrimination in education would include issues of generation, language,

and immigration status as well, but these topics are well beyond the scope of

this article. Therefore, despite problematizing the concepts of race and

ethnicity, and arguing for concepts that are socially constructed, historical,

relational, and variable, I yield to the practicalities of space and mission and

contradict key aspects of the above arguments. That is, I focus primarily on

black-white racial disparities, and when I label students, I use the five static

categories commonly employed by the federal government and most school

systems: Asian Americans, blacks, Hispanics, Native Americans, and whites.

RACIAL DISPARITIES AND DISCRIMINATION IN EDUCATION

During the 1970s and 1980s, the racial gap in educational outcomes

narrowed (Grissmer, Flanagan, & Williamson, 1998). Today, however,

racially correlated disparities in K-12 education are present in grades, test

scores, retention and dropout rates, graduation rates, identification for

special education and gifted programs, extracurricular and cocurricular

involvement, and discipline rates. The task of determining when educa-

tional disparities are caused by racial discrimination is complicated by the

close association of race with social class. 4Sorting out race effects from class

effects is extremely difficult, both conceptually and methodologically. 1056 Teachlers College Record

Reviews of recent large-scale studies suggest that at most, socioeconomic background explains 33% of the racial gap in education (Hedges & Nowell, 1998, 1999; Jencks & Phillips, 1998). This leaves a large portion of the variance in racial disparities in education that must be explained by factors other than social class differences among students of different races.

DISPARITIES

According to results obtained by the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), white, black, and Latino students in all assessment years (ages 9, 13, 17) show gains in mathematics between the mid-1 970s and the late 1990s. Whites and blacks show gains in science for 9 and 13 year old students, and Latino students show gains in science across all assessment years. Among blacks and Latinos, overall gains in reading appear at each age; for whites, reading gains are evident for 9- and 13-year-old students (Campbell, I-Jombo, & Mazzeo, 2000; Hedges & Nowell, 1998, 1999). The gains, however, conceal an important story. According to Michael Nettles, minority children have mastered the basics but not higher level skills (Hoff, 2000). NAEP results indicate that white students score higher in reading, mathematics, and science than do blacks and Latinos. According to Hedges and Nowell (1998, 1999) other national surveys indicate racial gaps comparable in size to the current differences found in NAEP: The black- white differences among 17-year olds range from 0.7 SD in reading to approximately 0.9 SD in mathematics. NAEP results suggest that racial gaps in test scores held steady among 9- and 17-year-old students but declined among 13-year-olds (Campbell et al., 2000). In short, across all three age cohorts in all three subjects, the smallest test gaps occurred in the 1980s; although the gaps of the 1990s were larger than in the previous decade, they were smaller than in the 1970s (Campbell et al., 2000; Gamoran, 2001; Hedges & Nowell, 1999).

SAT results reflect similar patterns. The College Board (2001) reports that SAT verbal scores are highest among whites (528) and lowest among blacks (434); SAT mathematics scores are highest among Asians (565, whites average 530) and lowest among blacks (426).

Other educational indicators show similar differences by race. Using Current Population Surveys, Hauser, Simmons, and Pager (2000) report that dropout rates are the lowest among whites and highest among Latinos. Since the 1970s, rates have declined among whites and blacks but not among Latinos. Blacks are more likely than whites to repeat a grade (Campbell et al. 2000) and to be placed in special education programs (Oswald, Coutinho, & Best, 2000), especially in school systems operating under court orders to desegregate (Eitle, 2002). Blacks, Latinos, and Native Americans disproportionately are found in lower tracks (Hallinan, 1998; When Are Racial Disparities in Education thle Result of Racial Discrimination? 1057

Lucas, 1999; Lucas & Berends, 2002; Mickelson, '2001; Oakes, 1985, 1994;

Oakes, Muir, & Joseph, 2000; Welner, 2001) where curricula and

instructional practices are weaker. Minorities are likely to attend schools

where they have access to fewer advanced placement classes than whites

(Pach6n, Federman, & Castillo, in press). Blacks, Latinos, and Native

Americans are more likely to learn in schools with fewer material and

teacher resources, a weaker academic press, and greater concentrations of

poor, homeless, limited English-speaking, and immigrant students (Kahlen-

berg, 2001; Lee, Burkam, & LoGerfo, 2001; Natriello, McDill, & Pallas,

1990; Van Hook, 2002).

IDENTIFYING WHEN DISPARITIES RESULT FROM DISCRIMINATION

As shown previously, the task of describing racial disparities is fairly

straightforward. Determining when they are caused by racial discrimination

is far more complex, however. We can all agree on simple instances of

discrimination-a racist teacher or a dual school system-but it is more

challenging to identify complex cases because they result from the

cumulative effects of institutions' and people's actions conditioned by

structure and culture and framed by history.

Under Jim Crow education, disparities in opportunities to learn and in

outcomes were caused by official racial discrimination against blacks, Native

Americans and (in some states) Asians and Latinos. Today, however, one can

argue that racially correlated disparities may or may not be due to racial

discrimination. For example, racial gaps in test scores are not necessarily

evidence of discrimination-unless there are systematic racial differences in

opportunities to learn (OTL) materials on the tests and in access to licensed,

experienced educators. Similarly, not all racial disparities in track

placement are necessarily discriminatory. Although decisions in track

placement decisions lie primarily with educators, the students and their

parents also are involved in the choices to varying degrees. Yet if

comparably able students of different races are placed systematically in

different tracks because educators teach, advise, or schedule blacks

differently than whites, we may conclude that discrimination is involved.

The existence of systematic racial disparities over time suggests the school

system engages in institutional discriminatory practices.

HOW SOCIAL SCIENCE ACCOUNTS FOR TI-IE RACIAL GAP IN

EDUCATION

In this article I focus on the not-so-simple manifestations of racial

discrimination and on the complex, often ambiguous processes that

generate them. To successfully distinguish when the race gap is more than 1058 Teachers College Record

a disparity and is discrimination, it is necessary to survey the explanations offered by social scientists for the gap. I organize explanations for the race gap in educational outcomes into six major categories: biological determin- ism, social structure, school structures and opportunities to learn, family background, culture, and the state.

BEHAVIORAL GENETICS

Periodically during the 20th century, theories that the racial gap is due to inherited, fixed racial differences in cognitive abilities circulated in the popular and scientific press Jensen, 1969; Terman, 1923). Herrnstein and Murray (1994) resurrected this argument with The Bell Curve, a book claiming that racial and social class differences in educational achievement, attainment, wealth, and poverty are the result of immutable genetic differences in cognitive ability. They recommended an end to what they regard as futile social welfare and educational programs designed to eliminate racial and class gaps in outcomes.

Decades ago, anthropologists established that the relationship between genetic diversity and raciaVethnic diversity is weak; diversity in the former does not map easily onto the latter because genetic variation within socially constructed racial groups is greater than between those groups (Marks, 2002b). Moreover, genetic variations among humans do not predict patterns of behavioral diversity (Marks, 2002c). The scholarship of biologists (Gould, 1981), molecular anthropologists (Marks, 2002a), cognitive psychol- ogists (Gardner, 1993; Myerson, Frank, Rains, & Schnitzler, 1998) and sociologists (Fisher et al. 1996; Sorensen & FHallinan, 1986) has discredited the putative scientific basis of biological determinist explanations for racial differences in academic performance.

SOCIAL STRUCTURAL EXPLANATIONS

Enduring inequalities in outcomes tarnish the significant accomplishments of American educational institutions. The United States is not unique in this regard: Blossfeld and Shavit (1993) examined 13 industrialized nations and found that the relative likelihood of graduating from secondary school and entering higher education for people from different socioeconomic back- grounds has remained essentially the same over the past several decades despite an enormous expansion of educational capacity in all 13 nations. In the Netherlands and Sweden, where overall social inequality declined, so did the effects of social background on educational attainment. In view of the relationship between race and social class, it is therefore necessary to consider the connection between social stratification and the racial gap in educational outcomes. Wlhen Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1059

During the past few decades, racial inequality in educational outcomes

has declined. But can we anticipate that the race gap will eventually

disappear? From a social stratification perspective, the answer is it depends,

because approximately one third of the racial gap is estimated to be due to

social class. Thus, even if racial discrimination in education were to be

obliterated, a racial gap still would exist because blacks (and most other

minorities) are poorer than whites.

Reproduction Tlheory

Social scientists offer a number of theories to explain the resilience and

durability of class differences in educational outcomes. One of the most

provocative is reproduction theory. Over 25 years ago, Bowles and Gintis

(1976) argued that in a capitalist society, schools are designed to reproduce

the class system by providing unequal education to children according to

their class of origin. Because the social relations of school (and the home)

correspond to the social relations of production, schools foster capitalist

structures of production and reproduce class inequities. Reproduction

theory has been criticized for undertheorizing race and gender, for failing

to show explicitly how schooling reproduces class inequality, and as

overdeterministic. Whatever its theoretical shortcomings, reproduction

theory offers a powerful analysis of the differential opportunities to learn

that exist along class lines.

Resistance Thleory

Resistance theory (Giroux, 1981; MacFarland, 2000; MacLeod, 1986; Willis,

1977) incorporate human agency into the processes by which schools

reproduce class inequality. Resistance theorists argue that youths respond to

the disjuncture they see between (on the one hand) promises of mobility

and social transformation through educational success and (on the other) a

stratified political economy in which class inequalities are daily realities.

Adolescents' partial understanding of this disjuncture results in conscious

challenges to schooling. By rejecting educational credentials, however,

resistant students foreclose any possibilities of upward mobility through

education. Like reproduction theory, resistance theory undertheorizes the

role of race and gender in social and educational inequality.

Maximally Maintained Inequality

Class inequality in education has not declined despite the increase in

educational access during the last century because dominant social groups 1060 Teaclhers College Record

have preserved their advantaged positions. Once disadvantaged groups

attain the educational credentials previously held by the dominant groups, the credential requirements for higher status jobs are raised. Social and educational inequalities are maintained because privileged groups are able to protect their advantages until all group members reach a given status (such as high school graduation or a BA degree). Then the credential requirements for status maintenance and mobility are ratcheted up to the next level, where the elites are better able to meet the new standards. Raftery and Hout (1993) theorize that these dynamics operate as a maximally maintained inequality (MMI) system.

Effectively Maintained Inequality

Lucas's (2001) theory of effectively maintained inequality (EMI) refines MMI. Lucas notes that the effects of social background operate in at least two ways: they influence who completes a given level of education if completion is not universal at that level, and they influence the kind of education people will receive within levels of education that are nearly universal (such as high school). Curricular tracking is a central mechanism

through which EMI operates. For example, EMI is the key reason why more highly privileged parents vigorously fight tracking reforms (Wells & Serna, 1996). The social background factors that determine whether students will continue schooling also effect where in the stratified curriculum they will be schooled. As they make the transitions from elementary school through higher education, their location in the stratified curriculum at any given point has implications for their curricular trajectories during the remainder of their educational careers. In these ways, Lucas concludes, advantages of social background effectively maintain educational privilege for children of the advantaged (pp. 1678-1681).

All four models describe how social structure intersects with schooling to generate racial discrimination in educational outcomes. This is the case

because social reproduction and mobility depend on not only the acquisition of educational credentials, both in terms of skills and certification, but also socialization into the relations of production. As I show in the following sections, discrimination influences racial differences in the opportunities to acquire these educational credentials.

SCHOOL STRUCTURE AND RACIALLY CORRELATED OPPORTUNITIES

TO LEARN

As Lee (2002) observes, we cannot hold schools responsible for the racial and class disparities in school readiness that are evident as soon as kindergartners walk through the classroom doors. The educational system Wlhen Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1061

is responsible, however, for the initial disparities by race that grow rather

than diminish with each year children attend school.

American school systems (almost 16,000) are highly stratified on

precisely the dimensions that are most related to student achievement.

The ways schools are stratified is documented in numerous scientific and

popular articles and books. School organization is a key contributing factor

to educational stratification. Possibly the most powerful recent popular

account of this inequality appears in Kozol's (1992) Savage Inequalities in

which the author describes the breadth and depth of racial and class

differences in schooling across the United States. In this section I focus

on the relationship between the racial gap and three aspects of school

organization: resources, racial composition, and tracking.

Resources

Whether money matters for school outcomes is a longstanding debate

dating back at least to the Coleman Report's finding that funding is not

closely related to achievement (Coleman et al., 1966). Although skeptics

remain unconvinced (Hanushek, 1994, 1996, 1997), a growing body of

research establishes that money does matter and that where and how the

money is spent is also extremely important (Ferguson, 1998a, 1998b;

Greenwald, Hedges, & Laine, 1994; Hedges, Laine, & Greenwald, 1994a,

1994b; Weglinsky, 1997).5

Not only bricks and books but also human resources, such as high-

quality, experienced, credentialed teachers instructing in their area of

expertise in small classes (Darling-Hammond, 2000; Ingersoll, 1999;

Lankford, Loeb, & Wyckoff, 2002) are directly related to money. Other

resources are indirectly related to fuinding levels, such as stable peers, active

involvement by parents, motivated peers who value achievement and who

share knowledge with classmates, and a school climate imbued with high

expectations. These are associated with the racial and SES composition of

communities (Kahlenberg, 2001).6

Students who attend resource-poor schools are disproportionately

members of minority groups (Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997; Lee, Burkam,

& LoGerfo 2001; Payne & Biddle, 1999). Given the system of public school

financing, which depends largely on property taxes, and in view of the racial

segregation in public and private housing markets (Powell, Kearney, & Kay,

2001), it is not surprising to find racial (and class) differences in school

financial resources and in the opportunities to learn that they purchase.

The key here is that blacks and other minorities are less likely than

whites to have equitable access to these critical resources both within

and between school systems. The racial discriminatory practices that 1062 Teachers College Record

generate and allocate resources inequitably to schools contribute to the racial gap in outcomes.

Schlool Racial Composition

The original social science rationale for school desegregation rests largely on claims that desegregation improves black youths' access to the higher- quality education more often provided to whites. Almost five decades after the epochal Brown v. Board of Education (1954) decision, there is little argument about the positive long-term effects of desegregation on minority students' status attainment, racial attitudes, and other life course indicators (Armor, 1995; Braddock & McPartland, 1988; Grain & Mahard, 1978; Hawley, 2002; Wells & Grain, 1994). Although the Coleman Report (Coleman et al., 1966) showed that academic outcomes were better for blacks who attended desegregated schools, until recently questions remained about the positive short-term effects of desegregation on minority youths' achievement (Armor, 1995; Cook, 1984; St. John, 1975). A growing body of recent research identifies the benefits of desegrega- tion and the harms of segregation. Grissmer, Kirby, Berends, and Williamson (1994) concluded, on the basis of comparisons of NAEP scores over time, that the significant increases in academic achievement of black students in some states and not in others were due, in part, to desegregation

during the 1970s andl980s. Hallinan's (1998) synthesis of the social science evidence identifies multiple positive effects of diversity on students' outcomes. A group of eminent social scientists filed an amicus curiae brief supporting desegregation in the 1992 Freeman v. Pitts case (Sch1ool Desegregation: A Social Science Statement, 1991). These researchers argued that when schools employ practices that enhance equality of opportunity, including the elimination of tracking and ability grouping, desegregation produces clear (albeit modest) academic benefits for black students and does no harm to white students.

Other recent empirical research offers further evidence of the harm of segregation (Bankston & Caldas, 1996) and positive academic outcomes from desegregated schooling (Brown, 1999). My own survey research reveals that the longer blacks and whites learned in desegregated schools and classrooms, the better are their academic outcomes (Mickelson, 2001, 2002a). I also found that many of the potential gains of desegregation were subverted by resegregation through tracking, even in "desegregated" schools.

Hawley (2002) synthesized the extant empirical literature on the effects of diversity on educational quality for the U.S. Department of Justice. He concluded that "students who have the opportunities to learn in schools that are populated by students from different races and ethnicities can have Whlen Are Racial Dispaiities in Education tle Result of Racial Discrimination? 1063

an education that is superior to that of students who do not have this

opportunity" (p. 1).

Even as America's schools resegregate (Orfield & Eaton, 1996; Orfield &

Yun, 1999;Yun & Reardon, 2002), several recent federal court decisions

have concluded that contemporary manifestations of school segregation are

not evidence of dual systems (Board of Educ. of Oklahoma v. Dowell, 1991;

Capacchtione v. Chlarlotte-Mecklenburg Schools, 1999; Freeman v. Pitts, 1992).

However, judicial standards of evidence are often quite different from social

science standards of evidence (Ryan, 2002), and, in my view, racially

discriminatory practices by the school boards generate de facto resegrega-

tion. These actions include drawing school boundaries in ways that

maximize racial homogeneity in schools, siting of new schools in white

suburbs rather than midway between black and white communities,

permitting greater numbers of advanced placement courses to be offered

in middle-class white schools, or allowing the better teachers unfettered

freedom to move to schools with less challenging, middle-class students.

Therefore any race differences in outcomes that can be traced to

resegregation-differential access to better teachers, safer schools, more

rigorous academic climates-are evidence of racial discrimination by a

school system.

Tracking

As Hallinan and Sorensen (1977) observed, students cannot learn what they

are not taught, no matter how highly motivated and how capable they are.

Numerous studies indicates that student in higher tracks-even less

academically able ones-learn more because they are exposed to broader

curricula and better teaching (Braddock & Dawkins, 1993; Darity,

Castellino, & Tyson, 2001; Finley, 1984; Gamoran & Mare, 1989; Hallinan,

2001; Mehan, Villanueva, Hubbard, & Lintz, 1996; Oakes et al., 2000;

Slavin, 1990). Research also shows that a critical component of the racial

gap in achievement is the relative absence of black students in higher-level

courses and their disproportionate enrollment in lower-level ones (Oakes,

1993, 1994a;Jencks & Phillips, 1998; Lucas, 1999; Mickelson, 2001; Oakes,

1990, 1993, 1994a 1994b; Wheelock, 1992).7

Tracking and ability grouping begin very early in children's school

careers (Alexander, Entwisle, & Lettgers, 1998; Kornhaber, 1997) and have

consequences that follow students throughout the course of their education.

The effects of ability grouping and tracking are cumulative: young students

who possess similar social backgrounds and cognitive abilities but who learn

in different tracks become more and more academically dissimilar each year

they spend in school. 1064 Teachers College Record

Central to tracking theory is the notion that track placement processes are objective, technical, and rational rather than subjective or arbitrary

(Kulik & Kulik, 1982, 1987). Any flaws in the execution of tracking are merely glitches in implementation, not system design (Hallinan, 1994). Rhiel Pallas and Natriello (1999) investigated the process of tracking. They dispel any remaining assumptions we may hold that tracking decisions are systematically objective and rational. They show that arbitrary and idiosyncratic placement decisions are common, and can be related to seat availability in classes or students' ascribed characteristics. A growing body of research (Baker & Stevenson, 1986; Kornhaber, 1997; Lareau, 1987; Lareau & Horvat, 2001; Lee, Smith, & Croninger, 1997; Lucas, 1999; Oakes, 1985; Useem, 1992; Wells & Serna, 1996) documents how privileged parents use their superior financial resources, knowledge, and social networks to ensure that their children are placed into the top academic trajectories. That schools and school personnel respond favorably to exercises of these race and class advantages is part of the problem.

The relationship between desegregation and tracking often is discussed

in terms of first- and second-generation segregation (Meier, Stewert, & England, 1989; Welner & Oakes, 1996). First-generation segregation generally involves the racial composition of schools within a single district;

second-generation segregation involves the racially correlated allocation of educational opportunities within schools, and typically is accomplished by tracking. Because tracking can undermine the potential benefits of policies such as busing which are designed to eliminate first-generation segregation, some courts have ruled since 1967 that it is unconstitutional for school districts to use tracking and ability grouping specifically to circumvent

desegregation at the school level (Hobson v. Hansen, 1967; People Whlo Care v. Rocl ford Board of Education, 1994). Even so, researchers find tracking is used to maintain an unofficial white track within a desegregated school (Kornhaber, 1997; Mickelson, 2001; Welner, 2001). For example, Eitle's (2002) nationwide study reports higher levels of racially identifiable

tracking of blacks into special education in districts under a court-ordered

desegregation plan than in districts not under such orders. Once again, in accord with my definition of discrimination, I conclude that racially correlated ability grouping and tracking practices result in racially discriminatory educational outcomes.

FAMILY BACKGROUND

Explanations for racial disparities in school outcomes that focus on family background fall into two broad categories. The first concerns characteristics of families, such as the number of children, the marriage status of parents, the number of adults in the household, wealth and income, and the adults' Wlhen Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1065

educational attainment. All of these indicators correlate with race. The

second category concerns social class dynamics. These dynamics are deeply

connected with the ways in which families interact with schools, how parents

socialize their children for schooling, and how parents participate in their

children's education.

Parents from different ethnic and social class backgrounds possess

different levels and types of the resources critical for their children's

education (Garcia, 2001; Guo & Van Wey, 1999; Hidalgo, McDowell, &

Siddle, 1990; Irvine, 1990; Phillips et al., 1998; Roscigno, 1998, 2000; Suter,

2000; Yonazawa 1997). Many social scientists find it useful to consider

families' resources as their human, financial, social, and cultural capital. In

my view, differences in families' capital resources contribute to racially

discriminatory outcomes because of the ways in which school practices and

school employees grant privileges to elite families who have them and fail

families who lack them.

Financial and Human Capital

Lee and her colleagues (Lee, Burkam, & LoGerfo, 2002) used a nationally

representative early childhood data set to demonstrate that race and class

inequalities in children's school readiness are evident when they first walk

through the kindergarten classroom door. The association between family

background and schooling was well known even when the Coleman Report

(Coleman et al., 1966) reaffirmed its importance. Families' financial capital

influences how well children eat, their health, and what kinds of books, if

any, are present in their homes. In addition to the necessities of life, money

purchases access to the best developmental preschools, tutors, computers,

psychologists who test for giftedness, and homes with quiet bedrooms for

doing homework. If one parent earns enough to support the family, the

other parent is free to volunteer in the child's school (this assumes a two

parent family). Parents' own human capital influences student outcomes as

well: for example, the more education parents have, the better able they are

to help with their children's homework. When schools are neutral with

regard to (or passively accept) students' differences in parental resources,

parents' resources (or lack there of interact with school organizational

structures and practices in ways that contribute to discrimination in education.

Social Capital

Social capital refers to student and parental knowledge about how 'to ask

and whom to ask in schools, networks of parents who share information on 1066 Teaclhers College Record

how to customize their children's educational careers, and shared

confidence and trust in school personnel (Baker & Stevenson, 1986;

Bourdieu & Passeron, 1977; Carbonaro, 1998; de Graaf, 1986; Hlilliard, 1989; Kerckhoff, 1996; Lareau & Shumar, 1996; Ma, 1999; Muller, 1998).8 Because of the ways in which public schools are organized, staffed, and

operate, the social capital of educated middle-class white families is more conducive to school success than is the social capital of less highly educated

ethnic minorities and/or poorer families.9 Bryk, Lee, and Holland (1993)

found that one reason for the success of Catholic schools is the social capital

shared by the students' families. Catholic schools' sense of community, trust,

common mission, and shared responsibility for all children contribute to every student's success.

CULTURAL EXPLANATIONS

Cultural Capital

Bourdieu's (1977) theory of cultural capital bridges cultural and structural

dimensions of family background. It illuminates the (dis)continuity between

the material and the symbolic elements of school and family cultures.

Bourdieu viewed cultural capital as a mechanism for transferring class advantage from one generation to another. According to Lareau and

Horvat (1999), Bourdieu's major insight into educational inequality is that,

because schools embody dominant cultural forms (expressed in dialect, demeanor, tastes, sensibilities, stock of elite knowledge), students who

possess more of the valued cultural capital fare better than do their otherwise comparable peers who possess less of this capital. A great deal of

research demonstrates the relationships between possession of cultural

capital and positive school outcomes (DiMaggio, 1982; Dumais, 2002;

Farkas, 1996; Lamont & Lareau, 1988; Lareau, 1987; Lareau & Horvat, 1 999).

Lareau and Horvat (1999) show that the value of cultural capital depends

heavily on the particular social setting, on parents' and students' skill at

activating their cultural capital resources, and on the reciprocal, negotiated

process by which social actors in schools respond to the activation and accord it legitimacy. Lareau and Horvat argue that these factors create

moments of inclusion or moments of exclusion for families. They show how

the historical legacy of racial discrimination-in conjunction with school structures and operations-makes it more difficult for black parents,

independent of their social class, to activate their cultural capital on behalf of their children. Wlhen Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result ofRacial Discrimination? 1067

Cultural Deprivation and Cultural Difference

Cultural deprivation theories (Lewis, 1966; Loury, 1985) propose that

school failures stem from values and norms that are ill suited for school

success (e.g., present verses future orientation). The insidious underlying

assumption is that students from nonmainstream (and by implication,

inferior) cultures are deprived. Policies based on cultural deprivation

theory seek to change the culture of students and their families so that their

behaviors better suit the demands of the school. Cultural deprivation

theories form the basis for much of the original compensatory education

movement but have been largely replaced with more benign theories of

cultural difference (Valentine, 1969).

Without implying the superiority of mainstream culture, cultural

difference theorists argue that racial variations in educational outcomes

exist because cultural practices and values of certain racial and ethnic

groups are more conducive to educational success (Bernstein, 1977;

Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994; Delpit, 1996; Heath, 1983). For example,

Caplan, Choy, and Whitmore (1991) identify a cohesive family and hard

work as the "core" values that form the basis of Southeast Asian refugees'

beliefs; these have enabled their youths to achieve well only a few years after

their arrival in the United States. It is not clear, however, that core

educational values of minorities who enjoy less success in school, such as

blacks and Latinos, are essentially different from those of ethnic minorities

who are more successful (for a somewhat different view, see Steinberg,

Brown, & Dornbusch, 1992).

Cultural difference theorists also argue that school problems are linked

to the failure of white-controlled educational institutions to incorporate

nondominant cultures into school culture, curricula, pedagogy, or

structures (Hallinan, 2001). Much of the multicultural education movement

is a response to this critique (Banks, 2003).

Oppositional Cultural Framework

On the basis of ethnographic data he collected after conducting fieldwork in

Stockton, California and elsewhere, Ogbu (1991) and Ogbu and Simons

(1998) argue for the existence of an oppositional cultural framework that

influences minority achievement. They argued that many black students, as

members of an involuntary minority group, often reject educational

achievement as an avenue for success. They perceive labor market

discrimination as a relatively permanent barrier that cannot be scaled

through education, and they develop alternative frameworks for "making

it." Cultural and language differences from whites become markers of a 1068 Teaclhers College Record

collective identity as an oppressed people. This perspective is expressed in many black students' pointed disdain for the alleged link between education and opportunity. Based on their collective history of discrimination, and the perception that schools are majority-controlled institutions, activities associated with school success (speaking standard English, carrying books, doing homework, studying for tests) come to be viewed as compromises of black social identity and group solidarity. In this way, the behaviors that lead to academic achievement come to be associated with "acting white" (Fordham & Ogbu, 1986; Ogbu & Simons, 1998).

Ethnographic and survey research by other scholars both challenges and confirms the presence and effects of an oppositional cultural framework among blacks (Ainsworth-Darnell & Downey, 1998; Carter, 2002; Cook & Ludwig, 1998; Downey & Ainsworth-Darnell, 2002; Farkas, Lleras, & Maczuga, 2002; Ferguson, 1998a, 1998b, 2001; Mickelson, 2002b; Tyson, 2002). Whether or not black students-and other nonimmigrant minority youths-possess or reject oppositional cultural norms hostile to school success and embrace academic achievement remains a subject of intense debate.

Abstract and Concrete Attitudes

Cultural values are reflected in people's attitudes. In earlier research I demonstrate that attitudes toward education and the future take two forms (Mickelson, 1989, 1990). The first form, abstract attitudes, is based on the dominant American ideology that holds that education is the solution to most individual and social problems. All adolescents value education in the abstract, but these values do not predict differences in academic achieve- ment. Achievement is affected by students' perceptions of the opportunity structure that awaits them. Concrete attitudes embody these perceptions which fluctuate according to the race, ethnicity, and social class forces that shape individuals' and groups' experiences in the American opportunity structure. Concrete, not abstract, attitudes predict achievement among all adolescents. By examining both abstract and concrete attitudes toward education in relation to high school achievement, I show how widespread consensus in educational values can exist across race and class lines while at the same time racial and class differences exist in actual school behavior and achievement.

A number of critics (Ainsworth-Darnell & Downey, 1998; Carter, 2002; Downey & Ainsworth-Darnell, 2002; Ferguson, 1998a, 1998b) incorrectly conflate concepts of abstract and concrete attitudes with Ogbu's (1991) notion of an oppositional cultural framework. Concrete attitudes oper- ationalize a related but earlier construct advanced by Ogbu (1978), namely When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1069

the job ceiling. In that earlier work he argued that children of castelike

minorities (those who were incorporated into American society against their

will) face barriers to job success despite possession of educational

credentials. Children witness the labor market discrimination faced by

their parents, siblings, and community members, and decide that putting

effort into education may not pay off. My abstract and concrete attitude

constructs test Ogbu's "job ceiling" hypothesis by examining how the effects

of truncated opportunities associated with the racially stratified labor

market shape minority adolescents' attitudes toward education and how

those attitudes influence their achievement.

Stereotype Threat

Steele (Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1998) examines how the cultural

stereotype of black intellectual inferiority affects the academic performance

of the most accomplished and most capable black students. He notes that

this stereotype is widespread and has become part of every black child's

stock of knowledge. Although awareness is not acceptance, Steele believes

that the stereotype, nevertheless, impairs academic performance among the

brightest and most academically motivated blacks. Using experimental data,

he demonstrates that if academically able black students are cued about race

before engaging in an intellectual task, their performance will be lower than

that of comparable blacks who did not receive such a cue. Given the salience

of achievement to high performers, the limited number of blacks in most

university classrooms, and the pernicious cultural stereotype of black

intellectual inferiority, Steele argues that "stereotype threat" inhibits blacks'

academic performance because the most able black students are reluctant to

engage fully in intellectual challenges lest they validate the stereotype

through trying and failing. Instead their anxiety unconsciously leads them

to disengage, and disengagement undermines performance. I return to

subjects of stereotype threat, oppositional cultural frameworks, and racial

discrimination in education later in the article.

THE STATE, SOCIAL POLICY, AND RACIAL GAPS IN SCHOOL OUTCOMES

As I have argued elsewhere (Mickelson, 2000), state actors make policy

decisions that generate, perpetuate, or ameliorate conditions and structures

responsible for racial disparities in education. Walters (2001) maintains that

throughout the United States, from the establishment of the common

school in the 19th century until the present, racial inequality in educational

opportunities has been an explicit policy of the state. She notes that not only

do elites have greater access to the state, through which they influence 1070 Teachlers College Record

policies, but also that certain policies continue to allow elites (primarily

middle class whites) to activate their private resources so as to stave off the

intended effects of contemporary equity-minded educational reforms. For example, reliance on local property taxes as the main source of school

finance and the sanctity of local school district boundaries were critical to establishing inequality within and between communities, and to maintain-

ing stratified schooling after certain educational policies shifted toward

greater racial equality of educational opportunity in the 1950s.

According to Walters (2001), the social and political processes that produce racially segregated housing and a racially stratified labor market

also generate racially segregated schools and racial inequality in the

distribution of school resources. State policies of concentrating public

housing for low-income, largely minority families in central cities (as

opposed to scattered-site public housing or mixed income communities)

affect the racial composition of schools. State policies of establishing or permitting resource inequalities within and between districts exacerbate

educational disadvantages facing the minority and poor children who are

concentrated in these resource-poor schools. Such policies compound

neighborhood disadvantages with school disadvantages (Massey & Denton, 1993; Natriello et al., 1990; Oliver & Shapiro, 1995; Powell et al., 2001;

Walters, 2001; Wilson, 1987).

State actors site schools and draw attendance zones that assist or hinder desegregation; they design and operate systems of ability grouping and

tracking; they operate schools and school systems so as to permit middle

class white parents to activate their race and class privileges. Outside the schools, state actors create and then sustain racially separate suburban

residential neighborhoods, and their policies concentrate and isolate poor

people of color in underserved public housing. State actors then employ residential proximity as the guiding principle for school attendance zones;

they generate reforms-such as high-stakes testing-whose harsh account-

ability outcomes affect whites, blacks and Latinos disparately (cf. Haney,

2000; Heubert, 2000a, 2000b; McNeil, 2000; Valenzuela, 1998), in part because these state actors often fail to ensure equitable distribution of

opportunities to learn the materials covered on the tests. Inequities in funding exist largely because state actors rely on property taxes to fund schools even though this method permits striking inequalities in resources, and hence, in opportunities to learn, based on race and class.

Choice

The policies and practices of school choice illustrate why the state shares

responsibility for creating and/or ameliorating racial gaps in achievement

discussed previously. The state (members of legislatures, school boards, and Wlhen Are Racial Disparities in Education thte Result of Racial Discrimination? 1071

other governing bodies) creates the enabling legislation, rules, and

regulations that permit school choice-in all its manifestations. The

literature on choice, race, and achievement is voluminous and contradictory

(Fuller & Elmore, 1996; Godwin & Kermerer, 2002; Henig, 1994; Peterson

& Hassell, 1998). According to advocates, the evidence shows that parental

choice (vouchers, charters, and magnets) leads to greater gains for minority

students; critics cite studies showing the opposite (Scott, in press). Aside

from the unresolved issue regarding the effects of choice on individual

students' outcomes, other important questions remain: What are the effects

of choice on school systems? Does choice exacerbate or ameliorate racial

inequalities in opportunities to learn within school systems? How does

choice affect students whose parents do not actively choose a school or a

program?

Choice theorists such as Chubb and Moe (1991) anticipate that choice will

create positive consequences for schools and school systems. They

hypothesize that good public schools ultimately will survive market forces

unleashed by choice. To date, however, there is no consistent evidence that

public or private school choice improves the achievement of students in

choice schools, nor that it improves educational practice in schools not

selected. Evidence from Chile (McEwan & Carnoy, 2000) and New Zealand

(Fiske & Ladd, 2000) suggests that nationwide choice systems create

disadvantages for students from ethnic minority and poor families. Perhaps

market-based school reforms like choice will prove neutral with respect to

the race gaps in school outcomes. Preliminary evidence does not sustain this

position; rather, early evidence suggests the opposite (Fiske & Ladd, 2000;

McEwan & Carnoy, 2000; Scott, in press).

THE LOCI OF DISCRIMINATION: JUNCTURES, TRANSITIONS,

AND EDUCATIONAL CAREER TRAJECTORIES

Beyond obvious acts of overt individual racism and de jure institutional

segregation, where does the responsibility of schools for racial discrimina-

tion begin? The previous review of social science explanations for the race

gap suggests the loci of discrimination are in the junctures and transitions

that contribute to students' educational career trajectories and the social

dynamics that unfold at these points. How do we make sense of the

organizational and institutional dynamics taking place at these loci? Lucas's

(2001) model of effectively maintained inequality (EMI) regimes provides a

useful guide for viewing the options, actions, structures, sequence, and

dynamics where race discrimination occurs in education.

Students' own agency and racial identity are shaped by, and interact with,

the culture and structure of the school. At each juncture and transition 1072 Teachers College Record

during R-12, education offers opportunities for students to take paths leading to very different educational trajectories. Students take different paths within each trajectory. The policies and practices by which students are sorted and selected for these paths, and the options students and their families perceive are critical to our understanding of how racial discrimina- tion occurs and how its effects cumulate at each transition in students' educational careers.

Oakes (2002) suggests that we envision educational careers as processes taking place at the intersection of two axes. The x-axis consists of community forces and the school environment. The y-axis consists of the students' educational trajectories with their discrete junctures, at which critical transitions are made between and within schools; these are the conditions in which students are embedded and through which they move as they make critical transitions during their educational careers. Community forces consist of the wealth and health of the neighborhood and the city, the suburb, or the rural area, its demographic composition, the collective educational history and culture of the community, and the nature of the family-school-community relationships. The school environment refers to the racial composition of the school, its material resources (e.g., funding) and human resources (e.g., access to qualified teachers), the academic press and other aspects of the school climate, and the organization of the school's opportunity structure (how the tracking and grouping practices operate).

Students' educational careers are conditioned by racial discrimination in housing and labor markets, and in public housing policies which result in isolated urban communities with high concentrations of low-income, poorly educated, minority families. Even though a majority of black and Hispanic students do not live in inner city ghettos and barrios, the majority of these students live in urban areas and are increasingly likely to attend segregated minority public schools. At the same time, the suburbanization of middle class Americans, particularly whites-facilitated by tax and transportation policies-results in more privileged students attending newer, better resourced schools with few peers from low-income, poorly educated families.

Similarly, the decisions and choices students, families, and educators make are conditioned by the quality of the teaching staff, the material resources of the school, the racial composition of students and teachers in schools, staff and student attitudes and beliefs about race and ethnicity, the racial composition of ability groups and tracks, the academic press and the school culture, and whether the home-school relationship is inclusive and welcoming or exclusive and rebuffing.

In many respects, the trajectories of students' educational careers are launched with the critical transition from the home to kindergarten (or to Wizen Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1073

preschool). At each subsequent juncture, discrimination can affect the next

stage of the trajectory as racial disadvantage or racial privilege cumulates. Is

safe, high quality, affordable childcare in a development preschool available

in all communities? As the child moves from the home and preschool to

kindergarten, is the public school one of high quality? How ready to learn is

the child, and how does the school respond to this level of readiness? Which

children are assigned to the certified teachers? Lee and Burkam (2002) and

Lee et al. (2001) have shown that the poorest, least prepared minority

children systematically are assigned to the least prepared instructors in the

poorest quality schools; this is one reason why the academic effects of

developmental preschool, such as Head Start, do not endure throughout

elementary school years (although results of High/Scope's Perry Preschool

Project [Barnett, 1996, and Schweinhart, Barnes, & Weikart, 1993] suggest

otherwise).

Once in elementary school, is the child identified as eligible for gifted or

special education? Identification for either of these categories launches

students onto paths that often become master statuses. Once a student

enters a gifted track, the additional instruction and enriched curricula-not

to mention the self-fulfilling dynamics of labels working through teachers'

and parents' expectations-actually create a student who knows more and

can do more than comparable students not in the gifted track. The opposite

tends to be true for those in special education.

The gifted identification process is often racially discriminatory because

of what schools do and do not do. Across the country, whites are more likely

than blacks to be identified as gifted, and blacks are referred disproportio-

nately to special education. This is true in part because schools permit savvy,

privileged parents to deploy their resources on their child's behalf.1 0 In

addition, schools selectively offer parents critical information about private

testing options, or inform them that uncertified students still can take a

gifted class. The schools continue to use identification procedures that do

not capture the multiple forms of intelligence.

The transition to middle school is another critical juncture in students'

educational careers. The academic tracks and courses into which students

are placed exert a powerful effect on their present and future opportunities

to learn. Middle school tracks determine the parameters of the formal

curricula to which students are exposed, the stock of official knowledge they

are likely to acquire, the peers with whom they interact, the test scores they

will make, and the probable trajectory of their remaining education.

Although parents may play an active role, the literature identifies how

counselors, teachers, and administrators have enormous influence over

tracking, and how race and class forces influence the placement process.

When we find that placements are correlated with race and social class, we

should not be surprised. 1074 Teachers College Record

Race and class forces also contribute to students' construction of their own social identities, including their identities as learners and as members of a particular race, gender, and class. The structure of opportunity in the school (are top tracks virtually all white while the lower ones are disproportionately black?) interacts with students' complex social identities. Among whites, the racial stratification of school structures signals their privilege; among minorities, it may cue oppositional attitudes or stereotype threat that contribute to racial discrimination in education. What and where students learn in a given year is reflected in part by their test scores and grades. A given year's academic performance then becomes part of the conditions for learning the following year. The effects of this dynamic cumulate during middle school careers and launch students onto disparate academic pathways in high school.'

The transition from middle school to high school (or out of school) is another critical juncture. Again, the courses and tracks into which students are placed (a process in which counselors, teachers, and administrators continue to be influential) exert a powerful effect on opportunities to learn in high school. At this point in students' educational careers, stratified opportunities to learn within high school become gateways to the stratified outcomes that follow secondary school.

A CONCLUSION AND REFORMULATION

The core conceptual question guiding this article is "When are racial disparities in education the result of racial discrimination?" I have arrived at three answers. The first is a definition I derived from my review of the social science literature: racial discrimination in education arises from actions of individuals as state actors or institutions, attitudes and ideologies, or processes that systematically treat students from different raciaVethnic groups disparately and/or inequitably.

The second answer is that distinguishing racial discrimination from racial disparities is, in fact, an interesting intellectual, legal, and statistical challenge with possibly important policy implications. But in the everyday lives of black and white students (as well as Latino, Asian, Native American, and biracial youth), this distinction probably is less meaningful than social scientists and policy makers might hope. Distinguishing racial discrimina- tion from racial disparities is akin to cleaning the air on one side of a screen door." In the end, the dirty air mingles with the clean and harms respiration; in the case of education, the cumulative and interacting effects of racial disparities and racial discrimination ultimately harm children. As the social science literature suggests, the racial gap is the result of complex, dynamic processes that cumulate over time. Most likely, racial Wfien Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1075

disparities at T, trigger racial discrimination at T2, and these phenomena

generate further disparities, and so on. For example, because minority

students tend to be concentrated in low performing schools, they are more

likely than white students to spend larger portions of classroom time on "kill

and drill" to raise their own and their school's standardized test scores. The

corollary is true as well: They spend less time on broader and deeper

curriculum coverage in social studies, science, and the arts, and in activities

that develop higher order thinking skills. This is an unintended

consequence of the intersection of high-stakes testing and school racial

segregation: Minority students are more likely to receive fewer opportu-

nities to develop higher order thinking skills or to be exposed to richer

curricula. Imagine the racial differences in learning after a few years of "kill

and drill" in lieu of the richer, deeper, and more engaging curriculum that

test preparation displaces. Is it important whether this phenomenon is due

to racial disparities or discrimination?

The third answer follows from the first two and is perhaps the most

important because it attempts a conceptual leap. Recall, as I stated earlier,

the central question I seek to answer is, "According to the social science

literature, when are racial disparities in education the result of racial

discrimination?" I propose the following' reformulation of the question:

"When are racial disparities in education not due to discrimination?"

To illustrate the value of this reformulation, let us consider two theories

that have been advanced to explain the black-white race gap in educational

outcomes: stereotype threat and oppositional cultural framework. Both

theories make the agency of black students the core of black under-

performance. I argue that racial discrimination in education, in fact,

structures and conditions the exercise of that agency by black youths.

Under what historical conditions and contemporary circumstances do

certain groups of people systematically disqualify themselves from educa-

tional achievement? Willis (1977) described very clearly how past and

present came together in the working-class white British adolescent males'

lived culture, a culture that penetrated the emptiness of a postindustrial

society's promise of better economic prospects through schooling. The

"lads" exercised their agency in rejecting school, but in doing so they

precluded upward mobility based on educational credentials.

Black students make choices if they stifle academic achievement because

they care whether their peers feel that doing well in school compromises

their black identity. What leads some blacks to construct academic effort, the

use of standard English, or earning good grades as threats to their social

identity as black people? Individual black students exercise their agency

when they silence themselves in an AP classroom or disidentify with

academic achievement in response to a race cue before testing. What

transforms spotlight anxiety into a fear that, given their full effort to 1076 Teaclhers College Record

achieve, anything less than a brilliant performance will validate the stereotype of black intellectual inferiority and smear The Race?

The answer is racial discrimination manifested in the collective historical

experience of blacks in America since slavery including decades of lynchings, the Tuskegee syphilis experiment, and dual school systems; in

contemporary racist stereotypes and representations such as those in The

Bell Curve or the Senate's former majority leader's wistful yearning for the Dixiecrat Party's segregationist agenda; in racially discriminatory treatment

of people of color by the judicial system, the electoral system, the health

care system, the labor market, the housing market, even the supermarket,

and-not least-in the isolation of the chilly, white top academic tracks of

most high schools and flagship university campuses, an isolation which

signals the "otherness" of blacks, Latinos, and Native Americans.

Let me return to the reformulated question: When are racial disparities

in education not due to discrimination? Although polemical, this reformula-

tion offers several advantages over the original. First, it reflects the weight of the historical and social scientific evidence and acknowledges that there is no impermeable membrane between schools and the larger society. As

observed by Alexander (2001), so long as race confers privileges outside schools, it is hard to imagine that it does not do the same within schools. Second, it removes the justification for waiting to ameliorate the race gap

until we understand the precise mechanisms by which racial discrimination

in education operates. And third, if our question seeks to distinguish when

disparities are not the result of discrimination, we are more likely to develop

answers that address more of the forces that create and sustain the racial

gap in education.

Even if we conclude that discrimination does not cause racial disparities

in education, a conclusion I do not support, we cannot and should not

conclude that schools have no role in addressing educational disparities.

Public education is the only institution that can touch the lives of all students

in this nation. If public schools do not address educational disparities, then who or what institution will?

Notes

I An earlier version of this article was prepared for the Workshop on Measuring Racial Disparities (and Discrimination) in Elementary and Secondary Education, National Research Council Committee on National Statistics Center for Education,July 1, 2002, Washington, DC. The author is grateful to Valerie Lee, Samuel R. Lucas, and Stephen Samuel Smith for their incisive comments on earlier drafts. 2 A number of scholars and educators point out positive aspects of segregated black educational institutions under Jim Crow (Anderson, 1988; Walker, 1999; Watkins, 2001). It is important, however, both to acknowledge the genuine strengths while avoiding romantidzing When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1077

segregated black schools. To do otherwise is to substitute an idealized conception of segregated

education for the harsh realities of grossly inferior opportunities to learn that characterized

racially isolated black schools in the past. 3 In the article's review of social science research on racial disparities and discrimination

in education, I drew heavily from a 2001 special issue of Sociology of Education. I wish to

acknowledge my debt to the many scholars wvhose work appears in that issue.

4 Gender further complicates the relationship betveen race, class, and educational

disparities.

5 Another aspect of funding inequities is a consequence of the social class homogeneity

typically found within elementary school boundaries. Schools typically raise additional money

by sending students into their neighborhoods, churches, and extended families to sell candy,

fruit, or wrapping paper. Schools use these funds to supplement instructional supplies and to

support cocurricular activities. Some middle-class schools are able to raise enormous sums of

money, whereas schools in poorer neighborhoods in the same school system raise a fraction of

the funds available to more prosperous schools so that advantages accrue to students who

already have them (Lucas, 1999). This dynamic is what Merton (1968) called the Matthew

Effect after a passage in the New Testament, "For whosoever hath, to him shall be given, and he

shall have more abundance: but whosever hath not, from him shall be taken away even that he

hath" (Matthew 13:12, New KingJames Version).

6 The vast literature on teacher expectations indicates the importance of high

expectations for academic performance and how race intervenes in teachers' expectations

for particular students (cf. Ferguson, 1998b; Jussim & Eccles, 1992; Jussim, Eccles, & Madon,

1996). 7 The literature is inconsistent as to whether blacks are less likely to be in higher level

tracks, net of family background, effort, and ability. My findings from Charlotte are comparable

to those of Oakes (1993) and Weiner (2001) who also found that race affects academic track

placement. One likely reason why Oakes, Weiner, and I found significant effects of race on

placement even after controlling for SES, effort, gender, prior achievement, and so on is that

our studies used samples (or populations) from entire school districts. I suspect that the use of

large national data sets, which sample only a few pupils from each of thousands of school

districts, masks district-level effects that do not become clear until all students and all schools

are included in analyses. My analyses consider track placement dedsions in relation to their

actual educational contexts.

8 According to a New York Times article, former Citigroup securities analyst Jack

Grubman, sent an e-mail message suggesting he swapped a favorable stock rating for help (in

the form of a Sl million donation from his boss, Sanford Weill) getting his 2-year-old twins into

the exdusive 92nd Street Y Nursery School (Goldman, 2003, p. 22). The article explains that

New York's top-tier nurseries can be feeders to the "right" kindergartens, and then Trinity and

Dalton, and then Harvard. The top-tier nurseries provide superior education from on-staff

child psychologists, movement and music specialists, artists in residence, custom-tailored

programs and computers, small classes, highly educated teachers and administrators with

degrees in education and early childhood education. Moreover, these Baby Ivies offer their

young students the "right" social element.

9 Smith and Kulynych (2002) argue that applying the term social capital to a wide array of

social and political relationships, processes, and phenomena obscures distinctions crudal to

many political traditions. Moreover, this term carries heavy ideological baggage that obscures

the causes of, and possible cures of urban decline; facilitates depoliticized political discourse; is

inimical to efforts to develop deliberative democratic participation; and impedes efforts to

consider sodal relations in terms other than those of capitalism and the market. Although I

acknowledge the analytic and ideological baggage of the term social capital, I continue to use it

as a bow to contemporary usage and to locate my work within a certain scholarly literature. 1078 Teachers College Record

10 I am not proposing that schools ignore or hinder the efforts of parents to use their resources on behalf of their children. Instead, I am critiquing school policies and practices permitting privileged children to benefit from their parents' resources while ignoring the inequities these informal arrangements create vis-a-vis less privileged children. 11 This observation was inspired by Anyon's (1997) priceless metaphor about the futility of school reform without concomitant reforms of the political economies of the communities in whiclh the schools exist.

References

Ainsworth-Darnell, J. W., & Downey, D. B. (1998). Assessing racial/ethnic differences in school performance. American Sociological Review, 63, 536-553. Alexander, K. (2001). The clouded crystal ball: Trends in educational stratification. Sociology of Education, 34, 169-177. Alexander, K., Entwisle, D., & Lettgers, N. (1998, August). On the multiple faces of first grade tracking. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association, San Frandsco, CA. Anderson,J. D. (1988). The education of Blacks in the South, 1960-1935. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press. Anyon,J. (1997). Ghe(to schooling: A political economny of school reform. New York: Teachers College Press. Armor, D. (1995). Forcedjustice. London: Cambridge University Press. Baker, D., & Stevenson, D. (1986). Mothers' strategies for children's school achievement: Managing the transition to high school. Sociology of Education, 59, 171-182. Banks,J. in press. Handbook of research on multicultural education (2nd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey- Bass. Bankston, C., & Caldas, S. (1996). Majority African American schools and social injustice: The influence of de facto segregation on academic achievement. Social Forces, 75, 535-556. Barnett, W. S. (1996). Lives in the balance. Age-27 benefit-cost analysis of the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press. Bernstein, B. (1977). Class, codes, and control (Vol. 1). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Bialystok, A., & H-lakuta, K. (1994). In other words. New York: Basic Books. Blossfeld, H.-P., & Shavit, Y. (1993). Persistent inequality: Changing educational attainment in thirteen countries. Boulder, CO: Westview. Board of Education of Oklahomna City v. Dowell, 498 U.S. 237 (1991). Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice (R. Nice, Trans.). London: Cambridge University Press. Bourdieu, P., & Passeron, J. (1977). Reproduction in educatian, society, and culture. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (1976). Schooling in capitalist America. New York: Free Press. Braddock, J., & Dawkins, M. (1993). Ability grouping, aspirations, and attainments. Journal of Negro Education, 62, 324-336. Braddock, J., & McPartiand, J. M. (1988). The social and academic consequences of school desegregation. Equity and Choice, 4 (2), 50-73. Brown v. Board of Education, 347 U.S. 483 (1954). Brown, S. (1999, August). High school racial composition: Balancing excellence and equity. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Sociological Assodation, Washington, DC. Bryk, A., Lee, V., & Holland, P. (1993). Catholic schools and the common good. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1079

Campbell, J. R., Hombo, C. M., & Mazzeo, J. (2000). NAEP 1999 trends in academic progress:

Three decades of student perfornance (NCES 2000-469). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of

Education.

Capacchione et al. v. Charlotte-Mecklenburg Sclools, 57 E Supp.2d 228 (W.D.N.C. 1999).

Caplan, N., Choy, M., & Whitmore,J. (1991). Children of the Boat People. Ann Arbor: University

of Michigan Press.

Carbonaro, W. (1998). A little help from my friend's parents. Intergenerational closure and

educational outcomes. Sociology of Education, 64, 199-215.

Carter, P. (2002). Low-income Black and Latino youths' mobility orientations: Aspirations, culture, and

resistance to 'acting White'. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.

Chubb, J., & Moe, T. (1991). Politics, markets, and America's schools. Washington, DC: Brookings.

Clodfelter, C., Ladd, H., & Vigdor, J. (2002, August). Within school segregation: The case of North

Carolina. Paper presented at the Resegregation of Southern Schools Conference, Chapel

Hill, NC. Coleman, J. (1994). A vision for sociology. Society, 32, 29-34.

Coleman, J., Campbell, E. Q., Hobson, C., McPartland, J., Mood, A., Winfield, E, & York, R.

(1966). Equality of educational opportunity. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.

College, Board (2001). College Board Seniors national report. New York: Author.

Cook, P.J., & Ludwig,J. (1998). The burden of 'acting White': Do Black adolescents disparage

academic achievement? In C. Jencks & M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black-White test score gap (pp.

375-401). Washington, DC: Brookings.

Cook, T. (1984). School desegregation and black achievement. Washington, DC: US Department of

Education. Cousins, L. H. (2002). Playing between classes: Exploring class, race, and culture in a Black high school

and community. Unpublished manuscript.

Crain, R., & Mahard, R. E. (1978). Desegregation and black achievement: A review of the

research. Law and Contemporaiy Problems, 43, 17-58.

Darity, W., Castellino, D., & Tyson, K. (2001, May). Increasing opportunity to learn via access to

rigorous courses and programs: One strategy for closing the achievemnent gap for at risk and ethnic

minority students. Raleigh, NC: State Board of Education.

Darling-Hammond, L. (2000). Teacher quality and student achievement: A review of state

policy evidence. Education PolicyAnalysis Archives, 8, 1-50. RetrievedJanuary 28, 2001, from

http:l/epaa.edulepaalv8 nl.

de Graaf, P. M. (1986). The impact of financial and cultural resources on educational

attainment in the Netherlands. Sociology of Education, 59, 237-246.

Delgado, R., & Stefancic, J. (2001). Critical race theory. New York: New York University Press.

Delpit, L. (1996). Other people's children. New York: Free Press.

DiMaggio, P. (1982). Cultural capital and school success: The impact of status culture

participation on grades of U.S. high school students. American Sociological Review, 47,

189-201. Downey, D. B., & Ainsworth-Darnell,J. W. (2002). Reply: The search for oppositional culture

among Black students. American Sociological Review, 67, 156-164.

Dumais, S. A. (2002). Cultural capital, gender, and school success: The role of habitus. Sociology

of Education, 75, 44-68. Duncan, G., & Brooks-Gunn,J. (1997). Consequences of growing up poor. New York: Russell Sage

Foundation.

Eitle, T. M. (2002). Special education or racial segregation: Understanding variation in the

representation of Black students in educable mentally handicapped programs. The

Sociological Quarterly, 43, 575-605.

Farkas, G. (1996). Human capital or cultural capital: Ethnicity and poverty groups in an urban school

district. New York: Aldine. 1080 Teachers College Record

Farkas, G., Lleras, C., & Maczuga, S. (2002). Comment: Does oppositional culture exist in minority and poverty peer groups? American Sociological Review, 67, 148-155. Ferguson, R. R (1998a). Can schools narrow the Black-White test score gap? In C. Jencks, & M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black-White test score gap (pp. 318-374). Washington, DC: Brookings. Ferguson, R. R (1998b). Teachers' perceptions and expectations and the Black-White test score gap. In C. Jencks & M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black-White test score gap (pp. 273-317). Washington, DC: Brookings. Ferguson, R. F. (2001). A diagnostic analysis of Black-White CPA disparilies in Shaker Heights, Ohio ((Brookings Papers on Education Policy). Washington, DC: Brookings. Finley, M. (1984). Teachers and tracking in a comprehensive high school. Sociology of Education, 57, 233-243. Fisher, C., Hout, M.,Janowski, M., Lucas, S., Swidler, A., & Voss, K. (1996). Inequality by design: Cracking the Bell Curve myth. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Fiske, E., & Ladd, H. (2000). When sciools compete: A cautionary tale. Washington, DC: Brookings. Fordham, S. (1996). Blacked out: Dilemnas of race, identity, and success at Capital High. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Fordham, S., & Ogbu,J. U. (1986). Black students' school success: Coping with the burden of 'acting White.' The Urban Review, 18 (3), 176-206. Freeman v. Pitts, 503 U.S. 467 (1992). Fuller, B., & Elmore, R. (1996). Whio chooses? Who loses? New York: Teachers College Press. Gamoran, A. (2001). American schooling and educational inequality: A forecast for the 21st century. Sociology of Education,.34, 135-153. Gamoran, A., & Mare, R. (1989). Secondary school tracking and educational equity: Compensation, reinforcement, or neutrality. American Journal of Sociolog, 94, 1146-1183. Garcia, E. E. (2001). Hispanic education in the United States. Ratcesy alas. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield.

Gardner, 1-1. (1993). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books. Giroux, 1-1. (1981). Theory and resistance in education. South Hadley, MA: Bergin & Garvey. Godwin, K., & Kermerer, F (2002). School choice tradeoffs. Austin: University of Texas Press. Goldman, V. (2003 January 12). The Baby Ivies. Preschool pedagogy for up to $15,000. The New lbrk Times Magazine, 22-25. Gould, S. J. (1981). The mismeasure of man. New York: Norton. Greenwald, R., Hedges, L. V., & Laine, R. D. (1994). The effect of school resources on students' achievement. Review of Educational Research, 66, 361-396. Grissmer, D. W., Flanagan, A., & Williamson, S. (1998). Why did the Black-White score gap narrow in the 1970s and 1980s? In C. Jencks & M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black-White test score gap (pp. 182-228). Washington, DC: Brookings. Grissmer, D. W., Kirby, S. N., Berends, M., & Williamson, S. (1994). Student achievenent and the changing American family. Washington, DC: RAND. Guo, G., & Vanwey, L. K. (1999). Sibship size and intellectual development.American Sociological Review, 64, 169-187. Hallinan, M. (1994). Tracking: From theory to practice. Sociology of Education, 67, 70-91. Hallinan, M. T (1998). Diversity effects on student outcomes: Social science evidence. Ohio State Law Journal, 59, 733-754. Hallinan, M. T. (2001). Sociological perspectives on Black-White inequalities in American schooling. Sociology of Education, 34, 50-70. Hallinan, M. T, & Sorensen, A. (1977). The dynamics of learning: A conceptual model (Discussion Paper 444-77). Madison, WI: Institute for Research on Society I-laney, W. (2000). The myth of the Texas miracle in education. Education PolicyAnalysisArchives, 8, 41. Retrieved December 27, 2000, from http:llepaa.asu.edu/epaa/v8n41 When Are Racial Disparities in Education tie Result of Racial Discrimination? 1081

Hanushek, E. (1994). Money might matter somewhere: A response to Hedges, Laine, and

Greenwald. Educational Researcher, 23, 5-8.

Hanushek, E. (1996). A more complete picture of school resource policy. Review of Educational

Research, 66, 397-409.

Hanushek, E. (1997). Assessing the effects of school resources on student performance: An

update. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 19, 141-164.

Hauser, R. M., Simmons, S. J., Pager, D. 1. (2000). High school dropout, race-ethnicity and social

background from 1970s to the 1990s (CDE Working Paper No. 2000-12). University of

Wisconsin-Madison, Center for Demography and Ecology.

Hawley, W. (2002). Diversity and educational quality. Unpublished manuscript, University of

Maryland, College Park.

Heath, S. (1983). Ways with words. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Hedges, L. V., Laine, R. D., & Greenwald, R. (1994a). Does money matter? A meta-analysis of

studies of the effects of differential inputs on student outcomes. Educational Researcher, 23,

5-14. Hedges, L. V., Laine, R. D., & Greenwald, R. (1994b). Money does matter somewhere: A reply

to Hanushek. Educational Researcher, 23, 9-10.

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1998). Black-White test score convergence since 1965. In C.Jencks

& M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black-White test score gap (pp. 149-181). Washington, DC:

Brookings.

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1999). Changes in the Black-White gap in test scores. Sociology of

Education, 72, 111-135.

Henig, J. (1994). Rethinking school choice: Limits of the market metaphor. Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press. Herrnstein, R., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve. New York: Free Press.

Heubert, J. P. (2000a). High stakes. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Heubert, J. P. (2000b, November). High-stakes testing: Opportunities and risks for students of color,

English-language learners, and students with disabilities. Paper presented at the Conference on

Minority Issues in Special Education in Public Schools. Cambridge, MA.

Hidalgo, N. M., McDowell, C. L., & Siddle, E. V.(Eds.). (1990). Facing racism in education

(Reprint Series No. 21). Cambridge, MA. Harvard Educational Review.

Hilliard, A. (1989). Reintegration for education: Black community involvement with Black

students in schools. In W. D. Smith (Ed.), Black education: A quest for excellence. New

Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.

Hobson v. Hansen, 269 R Supp. 401 (D.C. Cir. 1967).

Hoff, D. J. (2000, September). Gap widens between Black and White students on NAEP.

Education Week, 20 (1), 6.

Ingersoll, R. (1999). The problem of underqualified teachers in American secondary schools.

Educational Researcher, 28, 26-37.

Irvine,J. J. (1990). Black students and schoolfailure: Policies, practices, and prescriptions. New York:

Greenwood Press.

Jencks, C., & Phillips, M. (Eds.), (1998). The Black-White test score gap. Washington, DC:

Brookings. Jensen, A. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harvard Educational

Review, 39, 1-123. Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1992). Teacher expectations II: Construction and reflection of student

achievemenLJournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 947-61.

Jussim, L., Eccles, J., & Madon, S. (1996). Social perception, social stereotypes, and teacher

expectations: Accuracy and the quest for powerful self-fulfilling prophecy. Advances in

Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 281-387. Kahlenberg, R. (2001, February 21). An unambitious legacy. Education Week, 20 (23), 48. 1082 Teachers College Record

Kercklioff,A. (Ed.). (1996). Generating social stratification: Toward a new research agenda. Boulder, CO: Westview. Kluger, R. (1977). Simtple justice. New York: Random House. Kornhlaber, M. (1997). Seeking strengtlhs: Equitable identification for gifted education and the theory of multiple intelligences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA. Kozol, J. (1992). Savage inequalities. New York: Crown. Kulik, C., & Kulik,J. (1982). Effects of ability grouping on secondary school students: A meta- analysis of evaluation findings. American Educational ResearclhJournal, 19, 415-428. Kulik, C., & Kulik, J. (1987). Effects of ability grouping on student achievement. Equity and Excellence, 23, 22-30. Lamont, M., & Lareau, A. (1988). Cultural capital: Allusion, gaps, and glissandos in recent theoretical developments. Sociological Theory, 6, 153-68. Lankford, H., Loeb, S., & Wyckoff, J. (2002). Teacher sorting and the plight of urban schools: A descriptive analysis. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 24, 37-62. Lareau, A. (1987). Home advantage. Newv York: Falmer. Lareau, A., & H-lorvat, E. M. (1999). Moments of social inclusion and exclusion: Race, class, and cultural capital in family-school relationships. Sociology of Education, 72, 135-153. Lareau, A., & Shumar, W. (1996). The problem of individualism in family-school policies. Sociology of Education, 29, 24-39. Lee, V. E. (2002,July). Discussant remarks. National Research Council Committee on National Statistics workslhop on Measuring Racial Disparities in Education, Washington, DC. Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (2002, April). Inequality at thle starting gate: Social background and achiievemnent at Kindergarten entry. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. Lee, V. E., Burkam, D. T., & LoGerfo, L. E (2001, April). Who goes wlhere? How Kindergartners' social background maps onto thle quality of the elemnentary sclhools thzey attend. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Seattle, WA. Lee, V. E., Smith,J. B., & Croninger, R. G. (1997). How high school organization influences the equitable distribution of learning in mathematics and science. Sociology of Education, 70, 128-150. Lewis, 0. (1966). The culture of poverty. Scientific Amnerican, 215, 19-25. Lopez, D., & Espiritu, Y. (1990). Panetlnicity in the United States: A theoretical framework. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 13, 198-224. Loury, G. (1985). The moral quandary of the Black community. The Public Interest, 75, 19-31. Lucas, S. R. (1999). Tracking inequality. New York: Teachers College Press. Lucas, S. R. (2001). Effectively maintained inequality: Education transitions, track mobility, and social background effects. American Journal of Sociology, 106, 1642-1690. Lucas, S. R., & Berends, M. (2002). Sociodemograplic diversity, correlated achievemnent, and defacto tracking. Unpublishied manuscript, University of California at Berkeley. Ma, X. (1999). Dropping out of advanced placement matlhematics: The effects of parental involvement. Teachiers College Record, 101, 60-81. MacFarland, D. (2000). Student resistance: How formal and informal organization of classrooms facilitate everyday forms of student defiance. American Journal of Sociology, 107, 612-678.

MacLeod, J. (1986). Ain't no makin' it. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Marks,J. (2002a). Folk heredity. lnJ. Fish (Ed.), Race and intelligence: Separating sciencefrom myth (pp. 95-116). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum. Marks, J. (2002b). Anthropology and the Bell Curve. In H. Gusterson & C. Besteman (Eds.), On deeper reflection: Whiy America's top pundits are wrong about the world. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Wizen Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1083

Marks, J. (2002c). Whiat it means to be 98% chtimpanzee. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Massey, D., & Denton, M. (1993). American apartheid. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University

Press.

McCarthy, C., & Crichlow, W. (1993). Race identity and representation in education. New York:

Routledge.

McEwan, P., & Carnoy, M. (2000). The effectiveness and efficiency of private schools in Chile's

voucher system. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 22, 213-240.

McNeil, L. (2000). Contradictions of schzool reform: Educational costs of standardized testing. New

York: Routledge.

Mehan, H., Villanueva, I., Hubbard, L., & Lintz, A. (1996). Constructing sclhool success: The

consequences of untracking low aclhieving students. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Meier, K. J., Stewert, J. J., & England, R. E. (1989). Race, class, and education: The politics of

second-generation discrimination. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

Merton, R. K. (1968). The Matthew effect in science. Science, 159, 56-63.

Mickelson, R. A. (1989). Why does Jane read and write so well? The anomaly of women's

achievement. Sociology of Education, 62, 43-67.

Mickelson, R. A. (1990). The attitude-achievement paradox among Black adolescents. Sociology

of Education, 63, 44-61.

Mickelson, R. A. (Ed.). (2000). Clhildren on the streets of thie Americas: Globalization, homelessness, and

education in thle United States, Brazil, and Cuba. New York: Routledge.

Mickelson, R. A. (2001). Subverting Swann: First- and second- generation segregation

in the Charlotte-Mecklenburg schools. American Educational Researcht Journal, 38,

215-252.

Mickelson, R. A. (2002a). Segregation and the SAT Unpublished manuscript, University of

North Carolina at Charlotte.

Mickelson, R. A. (2002b, August). The role of abstract, concrete, and oppositional attitudes of

adolescents' academic achievement. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Sociological

Association, Chicago.

Muller, C. (1998). Gender differences in parental involvement and adolescents' math

achievement. Sociology of Education, 71, 336-356.

Myerson, J., Frank, M., Rains, F., & Schnitzler, M. (1998). Race and general cognitive ability:

The myth of diminishing returns to education. Psychological Science, 9, 61-64.

Natriello, G., McDill, E., & Pallas, A. (1990). Sclhooling disadvantaged children: Racing against

catastrophe. New York: Teachers College Press.

Oakes, J. (1985). Keeping track: How sclhools structure inequality. New Haven, CT: Yale University

Press.

Oakes, J. (1990). Multiplying inequalities: Thte effects of race, social class, and tracking on opportunities

to learn mathenatics and science. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.

Oakes,J. (1993). Ability grouping, tracking and within-sclhool segregation in the SanJose Unified Scllool

District. Diaz v. SanJose Unif/ed Sclhool District, 633 E Supp. 809 (N.D. Cal. 1985). Expert Report

to thle Court.

Oakes, J. (1994a) Ability grouping, tracking and witlhin-sclhool segregation in the New Castle County

Schools. Coalition to Save Our Clhildren v. State Board of Educ. et al., 901 E Supp. 784 (D. Del.

1995). Expert Report to the Court.

Oakes, J. (1994b). More than misapplied technology: A normative and political response to

Hallinan on tracking. Sociology of Education, 67, 84-88.

Oakes, J. (2002). Center X mission statemnent. Institute for Democracy Education, and Access

(UCLA/IDEA). Retrieved Nay 3, 2003, from http://wwwv.idea.gseis.ucla.edul

Oakes, J., Muir, K., & Joseph, R. (2000, May). Course taking and achtievement in math and science:

Inequalities that endure. Paper presented at the National Institute for Science Education

Conference, Detroit, MI. 1084 Teachers College Record

O'Connor, C. (2001). Making sense of the complexity of social identity in relation to adcievement: A sociological challenge in the new millennium. Sociology of Education, 34, 159-168. Ogbu, J. U. (1978). Minority education and caste. New York: Academic Press. Ogbu,J. U. (1991). Immigrant and nonimmigrant minorities in comparative perspective. In M. Gibson & J. Ogbu (Eds.), Minority status and schooling: A comparative perspective (pp. 3-36). New York: Garland. Ogbu, J. U., & Simons, H. D. (1998). Voluntary and involuntary minorities: A cultural- ecological theory of school performance with some implications for education. Anthropology &Education Quarterly, 29 (2), 155-188. Oliver, M., & Shapiro, T. (1995). Black wealth/White wealth. New York: Routledge. Omi, M., & Winant, H. (1986). Racialformation in the United States: From the 1960s to the 1980s. New York: Routledge. Orfield, G., & Eaton, S. (1996). Dismantling desegregation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Civil Rights Project. Orfield, G., & Gordon, N. (2001). Schools more separate: Consequences of a decade of resegregation. New research findings. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Civil Rights Project. Orfield, G., & Yun, J. (1999). Resegregation in American schools. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Civil Rights Project. Oswald, D. P., Coutinho, M. J., & Best, A. M. (2000, November). Community and school predictors of over representation of minority children in special education. Paper presented at the Conference on Minority Issues in Special Education in Public Schools, Cambridge, MA. Pach6n, H., Federman, M., & Castillo, L. (in press). High school race and class composition and advanced placement course availability. In A. Wells & J. Petrovich (Eds.), Putting equity back in school reform. New York: Teachers College Press. Payne, K. J., & Biddle, B. J. (1999). Poor school funding, child poverty, and mathematics achievement. Educational Researcher, 28, 4-13. People Who Care v. Rockford Board of Education School District No. 205, 851 F. Supp. 905 (N.D. 111. 1994). Peterson, P., & Hassell, B. (1998). Learning from school choice. Washington, DC: Brookings. Phillips, M., Crouse, J., & Ralph, J. (1998). Family background, parenting practices, and the Black-White test score gap. In C. Jencks & M. Phillips (Eds.). The Black-White Test Score Cap. (pp. 103-148). Washington, DC: Brookings. Powell, J. A., Kearney, G., & Kay, V. (2001). In pursuit of a dream deferred: Linking housing and education policy. New York: Peter Lang. Raftery, A., & Flout, M. (1993). Maximally maintained inequality: Expansion, reform, and opportunity in Irish education, 1921-75. Sociology of Education, 66, 41-62. Rhiel, C., Pallas, A., & Natriello, G. (1999). Rites and wrongs: Institutional explanations for the student course-scheduling process in urban high schools. American Journal of Education, 107, 116-154. Roscigno, V. J. (1998). Race and reproduction of educational disadvantage. Social Forces, 3, 1033-1061. Roscigno, V. J. (2000). Family/school inequality and African American/Hispanic achievement. Social Problems, 47, 266-290. Ryan, J. (2002. July). What constitutes discrimination in education? A legal perspective. Paper presented at the National Research Council Committee on National Statistics workshop on Measuring Racial Disparities in Education, Washington, DC. Schmidt, C. L. (2001). Educational achievement, language-minority students, and the new second generation. Sociology of Education, 34, 71-87. School desegregation: A social science statement: Amicus curiae brief of the NAACPet al., Freemnan v. Pitts, U.S. Supreme Court on writ of certiorari to the U.S. Court of Appeals for the Eleventh Circuit (1991). WMen Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? 1085

Schweinhart, L. J., Barnes, H. V., & Weikart, P. (1993). Significant benefits: Age-27 benefits of the

High/Scope Perry Presclhool Program. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.

Scott, J. (Ed.). (in press). Sclhool clhoice and student diversity: Examining the evidence. New York:

Teachers College Press.

Slavin, R. (1990). Achievement effects of ability groups in secondary schools: A best-evidence

synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 60, 471-499.

Smith, S. S., & Kulynych, J. (2002). It may be social, but why is it capital? The social

construction of social capital and the politics of language. Politics and Society, 30, 149-186.

Sorensen, A. B., & Hallinan, M. (1986). Effects of ability grouping on growth in academic

achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 23, 529-542.

Steele, C. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and

performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613-629.

Steele, C., & Aronson, J. (1998). Stereotype threat and the test performance of academically

successful African Americans. In C. Jencks & M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black-Wh ite test score gap

(pp. 401-430). Washington, DC: Brookings.

Steinberg, L., Brown, B. B., & Dornbusch, S. (1992). Ethnic differences in adolescent

achievement: An ecological perspective. American Psychologist, 47, 723-29.

StJohn, N. (1975). Sch ool desegregation: Outcomnes for ch ildren. New York: Wiley.

Suter, L. E. (2000). Is student achievement immutable? Evidence from international studies on

schooling and student achievement. Review of Educational Researcl, 70, 529-545.

Swann v. Clharlotte-Mecklenburg Sch ools, 402 U.S. 1, 15 (1971).

Terman, L. (1923). Intelligence tests and schlool reorganization. New York: World Book Company.

Tyson, K. (2002). Weighing in: Elementary-age students and the debate on attitudes toward

school among Black students. Social Problems, 80, 1157-1189.

Useem, E. (1992). Middle schools and math groups: Parents involvement in children's

placement. Sociology of Education, 65, 263-279.

Valentine, C. (1969). Culture and Poverty. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Van Hook, J. (2002). Immigration and African American educational opportunity: The

transformation of minority schools. Sociology of Education, 75, 169-189.

Valenzuela, A. (1998). Substractive sch ooling. New York: SUNY Press.

Walker, V. S. (1999). Tlheir highest potential. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press.

Walters, P. B. (2001). Educational access and the state: Historical continuities and

discontinuities in racial inequality in American education. Sociology of Education, 34,

35-149. Waters, M. (1990). Ethnic options: Ch oosing identities in America. Berkeley: University of California

Press. Watkins, W. H. (2001). Th e White arclhitects of Black education: Ideology and power in America, 1865-

1954. New York: Teachers College Press.

Weglinsky, H. (1997). How money matters: The effects of school district spending on academic

achievement. Sociology of Education, 70, 221-256.

Wells, A. S., & Crain, R. L. (1994). Perpetuation theory and the long-term effects of school

desegregation. Review of Educational Research, 64, 531-556.

Wells, A. S., & Serna, I. (1996). The politics of culture: Understanding the local political

resistance to detracking in racially mixed schools. Harvard Education Review, 66, 93-118.

Welner, K. G. (2001). Legal riglhts, local wrongs: Wh en community control collides with educational

equity. Albany: State University of New York Press.

Welner, K. G., & Oakes, J. (1996). A(Li) Ability grouping: The new susceptibility of school

tracking systems to legal challenges. Harvard Educational Review, 66, 451-470.

Wheelock, A. (1992). Crossing the tracks: How untracking can saveAAmerica's schools. New York: New

Press. Willis, P. (1977). Learning to labor. New York: Columbia University Press. 1086 Teachers College Record

Wilson, W. J. (1987). Thte truly disadvantaged. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Yonazawa, S. (1997). Making decisions about students' lives: An interactive study of secondary school students' academic program selection. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles Yun, J., & Reardon, S. (2002, August). Demographic overview: Trends in school segregation in thle Soutlh, 1987-2000. Paper presented at the conference on Resegregation of Southern Schools, Chapel Hill, NC

ROSLYN ARLIN MICKELSON is a professor of sociology at the University of North Carolina at Charlotte. With funding from the National Science Foundation and the Ford Foundation, Mickelson is investigating the equity effects of market-oriented reforms on students, particularly those from low- income and ethnic minority families. She is the author of "Subverting Swann: The Effects of First- and Second-Generation Segregation in the Charlotte-Mecklenburg Schools" (AERJ, 38: 215-252, 2001) and Children on the Streets of the Americas: Globalization, Homelessness, and Education in the United States, Brazil, and Cuba (Routledge, 2000).

COPYRIGHT INFORMATIONTITLE: When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of

Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective

SOURCE: Teach Coll Rec 105 no6 Ag 2003

WN: 0321300447006

The magazine publisher is the copyright holder of this article and it

is reproduced with permission. Further reproduction of this article in

violation of the copyright is prohibited.

Copyright 1982-2003 The H.W. Wilson Company. All rights reserved.